The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 22, Issue 1, pp 183–190 | Cite as

The Role of Rehearsal in Joint Control

Article

Abstract

Behavior analysts have offered accounts of the behavior involved in matching to sample and delayed matching to sample. But until recently have not offered a behavioral analysis of generalized matching-to-sample. The concept of joint control, however, seems especially suited to such an analysis The present study used a joint-control procedure to train five adult women to acquire a generalized sequencing behavior using an unfamiliar language. After joint-control training the participants were able to produce untrained picture sequences, and blocking the mediating response during the sequencing task resulted in a reduction in the number of accurate sequences. These results clearly support response mediation as a precurrent to various kinds of complex human behavior.

Key words

rehearsal joint control mediating responses blocking generalized matching naming 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bucher, B. (1975). Establishing discriminative stimulus control within generalized oddity performance without reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20, 38–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Cohen, L. R., Brady, J., & Lowry, M. (1981). The role of differential responding to samples in matching performance. In Michael Commons and John A. Nevin (Eds.), Quantitative studies of operant behavior: discriminative properties of reinforcement schedules. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  3. Lowenkron, B. (1984). Coding responses and the generalization of matching to sample in children. Journal for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Lowenkron, B. (1988). Generalization of delayed identity matching in retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 163–172.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Mayzner, M. S., Tresselt, M. E., & Helbock, H. (1964). Exploratory study of mediational responses in anagram problem solving. The Journal of Psychology, 57, 263–274.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Michael, J. (1985). Two kinds of verbal behavior plus a possible third. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 3, 2–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Parsons, J. A., Taylor, D. C., & Joyce, T. M. (1981). Precurrent self-prompting operants in children: “remembering.” Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 253–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Potter, B., Huber, S., & Michael, J. (1997). The role of mediating verbal behavior in selection-based responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 14, 41–56.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior, New York: Appleton-Century-CroftCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Zentall, T. R., Edwards, C.A., Moore, B. S., & Hogan, D. E. (1981). Identity: The basis for both matching and oddity learning in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 70–86.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.California State UniversityLos AngelesUSA
  2. 2.Applied Behavior Consultants, Inc.Rancho CucamongaUSA

Personalised recommendations