The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 22, Issue 1, pp 129–151 | Cite as

Joint Control and the Selection of Stimuli from Their Description

Article

Abstract

This research examined the role the two constituents of joint control, the tact and the echoic, play in producing accurate selections of novel stimuli in response to their spoken descriptions. Experiment 1 examined the role of tacts. In response to unfamiliar spoken descriptions, children learned to select from among six successively presented comparisons which varied in their color, shape, and border features. Repeated testing and training revealed that accurate selecting with new combinations of the same colors, shapes and borders, did not occur until after the children could themselves tact the individual color, shape and border features with the unfamiliar descriptions. Experiment 2 examined the role of self-echoics. Here, the stimulus features were given their familiar names, but the rehearsal of these names, while searching among the six successively presented comparisons, was impeded by a distracter task. Under these conditions selection of the correct comparison was found to depend on its position in the order of presentation. Correct comparisons presented earlier in the order, and presumably less effected by the distracter task, were more likely to be selected than correct comparisons presented later in the serial order. Taken together, these data suggest that generalized stimulus selection must be under joint tact/echoic control. The data also illustrate the distinction between mediated selection of a stimulus in response to its description (i.e., selection under joint control) and the traditional conception of an unmediated selection response evoked as a result of a heightened response probability in a conditional discrimination.

Key words

joint control verbal behavior conditional discrimination generalization delayed matching stimulus control children 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bickel, W. & Etzel, B. C. (1985). The quantal nature of controlling stimulus-response relations as measured in tests of stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 44, 245–270.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Cohen, L. R., Brady, J., & Lowry, M. (1981). The role of differential responding in match-ing-to-sample and delayed matching performance. In M. L. Commons & J. A. Nevin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: discriminative properties of reinforcement schedules. (Vol. 1, pp. 345–364) Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.Google Scholar
  3. Eckerman, D. A. (1970). Generalization and response mediation of a conditional discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 301–316.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Fields, L. (1978). Fading and errorless transfer in successive discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 30, 123–128.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Goldstein, H., Angelo, D., & Whetherby, B. (1987). Effects of training method and word order on adults’ acquisition of miniature linguistic systems. The Psychological Record, 37, 89–107Google Scholar
  6. Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Kendler, H. H., & Kendler, T. S. (1970). Developmental processes in discrimination learning. Human Development, 13, 65–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Lowenkron, B. (1969). Nonoutcome trial behavior: A predictor of solution shift performance and the effects of overtraining. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 484–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Lowenkron, B. (1984). Coding responses and the generalization of matching-to-sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 1–18.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Lowenkron, B. (1988). Generalization of delayed identity matching in retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 163–172.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Lowenkron, B. (1989). Instructional control of generalized relational matching to sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 293–309.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Lowenkron, B. (1991). Joint control and the generalization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–126.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical function of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Lowenkron, B. (2004). Meaning: A verbal behavior account. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 77–97.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Lowenkron, B., & Colvin, V. (1992). Joint control and generalized nonidentity matching: Saying when something is not. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 1–10.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Lowenkron, B., & Colvin, V. (1995). Generalized instructional control and the production of broadly applicable relational responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 12, 13–29.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Lowenkron, B. & Driessen, E. C. (1971). Solution mode in concept identification problems and magnitude of the overlearning reversal effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 81–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Michael, J. (1985). Two kinds of verbal behavior plus a possible third. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 3, 2–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Parsons J., Taylor, D. C., & Joyce, T. M. (1981). Precurrent self-prompting operants in children: “Remembering.” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 253–266.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Sidman, M. (1978). Remarks. Behaviorism, 6, 265–268.Google Scholar
  21. Sidman, M. & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Siegal, S., & Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonpara-metric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  23. Singh, N. N., & Solmon, R. T. (1990). A stimulus control analysis of the picture-word problem in children who are mentally retarded: The blocking effect. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 525–532.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Skinner, B. F. (1969). Behaviorism at fifty. In B. F. Skinner (Ed.), Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis (pp. 221–242). New York: Appleton-Century-Croft.Google Scholar
  25. Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  26. Touchette, P. E. (1969). The effects of graduated stimulus change on the acquisition of a simple discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 39–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Urcioli, P. J. (1985) On the role of differential sample behaviors in matching to sample. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 502–519.Google Scholar
  28. Wolff, J. L. (1967). Concept-shift and discrimination-reversal learning in humans. Psychological Bulletin, 68, 369–408.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.California State UniversityLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations