The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 22, Issue 1, pp 111–118 | Cite as

A Summary and Commentary on D. and S. Premack’s Original Intelligence

Article
  • 3 Downloads

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neuro-linguistics. Behavioral and Brain Science, 28, 106–167.Google Scholar
  2. Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D. & Cullinan, V. (2001). Education, pp. 181–195. In S. Hayes, D. Barnes-Homes., & B. Roche, (2001). Relational frame theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer/Academic Plenum.Google Scholar
  3. Bijou, S., & Baer, D. M. (1978). A behavior analysis of child development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  4. Butler, D. J. (2005). Adapting minds: Evolutionary biology and the persistent quest for human nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  5. Catania, A. C. (2001). Three types of selection and three centuries. Revista Internacional de psicologia y therapia psicologia, I(1). 1–10.Google Scholar
  6. Culotta, E. & Hanson, B., (2004). First words. Science, 5662, 1315–1335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
  8. Dugatkin, L. A. & Godin, J.G., (1992). Reversal of female mate choice by copying in the guppy (Poecilla reticulata). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 249, 179–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Epstein, R., Lanza, R. P., & Skinner, B. F. (1980). Symbolic communication between two pigeons (Columbia livia domestica). Science, 207, 543–545.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Epstein, R., Lanza, R. P., & Skinner, B. F. (1981). “Self-Awareness” in the pigeon. Science, 212, 695–696.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Fiorile, C. A. & Greer, R. D. (2005). The Induction of Naming in Children with No Echoic-to-Tact Responses as a Function of Multiple Exemplar Histories of Instruction. Paper submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  12. Gewirtz, J. L., Baer, D. M., & Roth, C. L. (1958). A note on the similar effects of low social availability of an adult and brief social deprivation on young children’s behavior. Child Development, 29, 149–152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Gillic, L. (2005). The development of naming in 2-year olds. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, New York.Google Scholar
  14. Greenwald, S. (1997). The human brain. London: Science Masters.Google Scholar
  15. Greer, R. D. & Keohane, D. D. (2005). The evolution of verbal behavior in young children. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 1, 31–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Greer, R. D., & Ross, D. E., (in press). Verbal behavior analysis: Developing and expanding complex communication in with children severe language delays. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  17. Greer, R. D., Singer-Dudek, J., & Gautreaux, G. (in press). Observational learning. Journal of International Psychology.Google Scholar
  18. Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-Valdez, C. (2005). The emergence of the listener to speaker component of naming in children as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 123–134.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Greer, R. D. & Yuan, L. (2003). Kids say the darnedest things. Paper presented at the International Conference of the Association for Behavior Analysis and the Brazil Association for Behavior Medicine and Therapy.Google Scholar
  20. Greer, R. D., Yuan, L. & Gautreaux, G. (2005). Novel dictation and intraverbal responses as a function of a multiple exemplar history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99–116.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Hart B. & Risely, T. (1996). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Life of America’s Children. NY: Paul Brookes.Google Scholar
  22. Hayes, S. Barnes-Homes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A Post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer/Academic Plenum.Google Scholar
  23. Holden, C. (2004). The origin of speech. Science, 5662, 1316–1319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Horne, P. J. & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Lamarre, J. & Holland, J. (1985). The functional independence of mands and tacts. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 5–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., Harris, D. S., & Randle, V. R.L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: Vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Michael, J. (1982). Skinner’s elementary verbal relations: Some new categories. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 1, 1–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Michael, J. (1984). Verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 363–376.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 191–206.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Moxley, R. A. (2004). Skinner’s adoption of Peirce’s pragmatic meaning for habits. Transactions of the Charles S. Pierce Society, XL(4), 743–769.Google Scholar
  31. Nuzzolo-Gomez, R. & Greer, R. D. (2004). Emergence of Untaught Mands or Tacts with Novel Adjective-Object Pairs as a Function of Instructional History. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 30–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Premack, D., & Premack, S. (2003). Original intelligence: Unlocking the mystery of who we are. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  33. Robinson, G. E. (2004). Beyond nature and nurture. Science, 304, 397–399.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Ross, D. E., & Greer, R. D. (2003). Generalized imitation and the mand: Inducing first instances of speech in young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 376, 1–17.Google Scholar
  35. Skinner, B. F. (1935). The generic nature pf the concepts of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology, 12, 40–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  37. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Skinner, B. F. (1987). Cognitive science and behaviorism. In B. F. Skinner, Upon further reflection (pp. 93–111). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  39. Staats, A. W. (1981). Paradigmatic behaviorism: Unified theory construction methods and the zeitgeist of separatism. American Psychologist, 36, 346–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Twyman, J. (1996). The functional independence of impure mands and tacts of abstract stimulus properties. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 13, 1–19.PubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Columbia University Teachers College and Graduate School of Arts and SciencesNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations