Advertisement

The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 22, Issue 1, pp 89–100 | Cite as

Antecedents and Consequences of Words

  • A. Charles Catania
Article

Abstract

As instances of behavior, words interact with environments. But they also interact with each other and with other kinds of behavior. Because of the interlocking nature of the contingencies into which words enter, their behavioral properties may become increasingly removed from nonverbal contingencies, and their relationship to those contingencies may become distorted by the social contingencies that maintain verbal behavior. Verbal behavior is an exceedingly efficient way in which one organism can change the behavior of another. All other functions of verbal behavior derive from this most basic function, sometimes called verbal governance. Functional verbal antecedents in verbal governance may be extended across time and space when individuals replicate the verbal behavior of others or their own verbal behavior. Differential contact with different verbal antecedents may follow from differential attention to verbal stimuli correlated with consequential events. Once in place, verbal behavior can be shaped by (usually social) consequences. Because these four verbal processes (verbal governance, replication, differential attention, and verbal shaping) share common stimulus and response terms, they produce interlocking contingencies in which extensive classes of behavior come to be dominated by verbal antecedents. Very different consequences follow from verbal behavior depending on whether it is anchored to environmental events, as in scientific verbal practices, or becomes independent of it, as in religious fundamentalism.

Key words

words verbal governance replication and selection attention shaping social contingencies scientific practice religious fundamentalism 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Baer, R. A., Detrich, R., & Weninger, J. M. (1988). On the functional role of the verbalization in correspondence training procedures. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 345–356.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Blackmore, S. (1999). The meme machine. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
  3. Catania, A. C. (1985). Rule-governed behavior and the origins of language. In C. Bradshaw (Ed.), Behavior analysis and contemporary psychology (pp. 135–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Catania, A. C. (1991). The phylogeny and ontogeny of language function. In M. Studdert-Kennedy (Ed.), Biological and behavioral determinants of language development (pp. 263–285). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Catania, A. C. (1994). The natural and artificial selection of verbal behavior. In K. Ono (Ed.), Behavior analysis of language and cognition (pp. 31–49). Reno, NV: Context.Google Scholar
  6. Catania, A. C. (1995). Higher-order behavior classes: Contingencies, beliefs, and verbal behavior. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26, 191–200.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  8. Catania, A. C. (2001). Three varieties of selection and their implications for the origins of language. In G. Györi (Ed.), Language evolution: Biological, linguistic and philosophical perspectives (pp. 55–71). New York/Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
  9. Catania, A. C. (2003). Verbal governance, verbal shaping, and attention to verbal stimuli. In P. N. Chase (Ed.), Behavior theory and philosophy (pp. 301–321). New York: Kluwer/Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Catania, A. C., & Harnad, S. (Eds.). (1988). The selection of behavior: The operant behaviorism of B. F. Skinner. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Catania, A. C., Lowe, C. F., & Horne, P. (1990). Nonverbal behavior correlated with the shaped verbal behavior of children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 43–55.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Catania, A. C., Matthews, B. A., & Shimoff, E. (1982). Instructed versus shaped human verbal behavior: Interactions with nonverbal responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 233–248.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Catania, A. C., Shimoff, E., & Matthews, B. A. (1989). An experimental analysis of rule-governed behavior. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 119–150). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  15. Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  16. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  18. Dinsmoor, J. A. (1983). Observing and conditioned reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 693–728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dinsmoor, J. A. (1995). Stimulus control. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 51–68, 253–269.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Greenspoon, J. (1955). The reinforcing effect of two spoken sounds on the frequency of two responses. American Journal of Psychology, 68, 409–416.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.Google Scholar
  22. Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Jaynes, J. (1976). The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  24. Kelleher, R. T., Riddle, W. C., & Cook, L. (1962). Observing responses in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 3–13.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Lovaas, O. I. (1964). Cue properties of words: The control of operant responding by rate and content of verbal operants. Child Development, 35, 245–256.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Palmer, D. C. (1996). Achieving parity: The role of automatic reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 289–290.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Palmer, D. C. (1998). The speaker as listener: The interpretation of structural regularities in verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 15, 3–16.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Risley, T. R. (1977). The development and maintenance of language: An operant model. In B. C. Etzel, J. M. LeBlanc & D. M. Baer (Eds.), New developments in behavioral research (pp. 81–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  29. Risley, T. R., & Hart, B. (1968). Developing correspondence between the non-verbal and verbal behavior of preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 267–281.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Rosenfarb, I. S., Newland, M. C., Brannon, S. E., & Howey, D. S. (1992). Effects of self-generated rules on the development of schedule-controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 107–121.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Rosenfeld, H. M., & Baer, D. M. (1970). Unbiased and unnoticed verbal conditioning: The double agent robot procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, 99–107.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour, 28, 1070–1094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Shimoff, E., & Catania, A. C. (1998). The verbal governance of behavior. In K. A. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in human operant behavior (pp. 371–404). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  35. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Skinner, B. F. (1969). An operant analysis of problem solving. In B. F. Skinner, Contingencies of reinforcement (pp. 133–157). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  37. Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Skinner, B. F. (1986). The evolution of verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 115–122.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Maryland, Baltimore County (UMBC)BaltimoreUSA

Personalised recommendations