The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 21, Issue 1, pp 123–134 | Cite as

The Emergence of the Listener to Speaker Component of Naming in Children as a Function of Multiple Exemplar Instruction

  • R. Douglas Greer
  • Lauren Stolfi
  • Mapy Chavez-Brown
  • Celestina Rivera-Valdes
Article

Abstract

We tested the effect of multiple exemplar instruction on the transfer of stimulus function for unfamiliar pictures across listener responses (i.e., matching and pointing) and speaker responses (i.e., pure tacts and impure tacts). Three preschool students, who were 3- and 4-year-old males and did not have the listener to speaker component of the naming repertoire, participated in the experiment. The dependent variable was numbers of correct responses to probe trials of both untaught listener responses (“point to__”) and speaker responses (tact and impure tacts) following mastery of matching responses for two sets of five unfamiliar pictures (Set 1 and Set 3). After each participant mastered matching (e.g., “match Labrador”) for Set 1 pictures they were probed on the three untaught responses to Set 1 words. That is, they were asked to point to Labrador, tact the picture of Labrador, and respond to the picture of a Labrador and the question “What is this?” Next, the participants were taught mastery of all four types of responses using MEI for a second set of five pictures (Set 2) and probed again on the 3 untaught Set 1 responses. Finally, matching responses were taught to mastery for a novel set of pictures (Set 3) and then probed on the three untaught responses. The results showed that untaught speaker responses emerged at 60% to 85% for two participants, and 40%- 70% for one participant. We discuss the role of instructional history in the development of the listener to speaker component of naming.

Key words

naming generative verbal behavior novel verbal behavior multiple exemplar experiences 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bahadorian, AJ. (2000). The effect of the learn unit on student’s performance in two university courses. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Columbia University.Google Scholar
  2. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Cullinan, V (2001). Relational frame theory and Skinner’s Verbal behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 69–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Becker, W. (1992). Direct instruction: A twenty-year review. In R. West & L. Hamerlynck, Design for educational excellence: The legacy ofB. F. Skinner (pp. 71–112). Longmont, CO: Sopris West.Google Scholar
  4. Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  5. Donley, C. R., & Greer, R. D. (1993). Setting events controlling social verbal exchanges between students with developmental delays. Journal of Behavioral Education, 3 (4), 387–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Emurian, H. H., Hu, X., Wang, J., & Durham, D. (2000). Learning JAVA: A programmed instruction approach using applets. Computers in Human Behavior, 16, 395–422CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fields, L., Reeve, K. F., Mateneja, P., Varelas, A., Belanich, J., Fitzer, A., & Shannon, K. (2003). The formation of a generalized categorization repertoire: Effects of training with multiple domains, samples, and comparisons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 291–313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Greer, R. D. (2003). Designing teaching strategies: An applied behavior analysis systems approach. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  9. Greer, R. D., Chavez-Brown, M, Nirgudkar, A., Steffi, L., & Rivera-Valdes, (July 2003). The effects of a procedure to teach fluent listener responses to students with autism and changes in the numbers of learn units students required to meet instructional objectives. Paper presented at the First Congress of the European Association for Behavior Analysis, Parma, Italy.Google Scholar
  10. Greer, R. D., & Keohane, D. (2004). The evolution of verbal behavior. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  11. Greer, R. D., Keohane, D. D., & Healey, O. (2002). Quality and applied behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst Today, 3 (1), 2002.Google Scholar
  12. Greer, R. D. & McCorkle, N. (2003). CABAS® international curriculum and inventory of repertoires from preschool through kinder garten. Yonkers, NY: CABAS® and the Fred S. Keller School.Google Scholar
  13. Greer, R. D., & McDonough, S. (1999). Is the learn-unit the fundamental measure of pedagogy? The Behavior Analyst, 20, 5–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Greer, R. D., Nirgudkar, A., & Park, H. (2003b). The effect of multiple exemplar instruction on the transformation of mand and tact functions. Paper Presented at the International Conference of the Association for Behavior Analysis, San Francisco, CA.Google Scholar
  15. Greer, R. D., Yaun L., & Gautreaux, G (2005). Novel Dictation and Intraverbal Responses as a Function of a Multiple Exemplar Instructional History. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99–116.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Hayes, S. C, Gifford, E. V, Wilson, K. G, Barnes-Holmes, D., & Healy, O. (2001). Derived relational responding as learned behavior. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relationalframe theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 21–49). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.Google Scholar
  17. Home, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Horner, R. D., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple-probe technique: Avariation on the multiple baseline. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 189–196.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Lodhi, S. & Greer, R.D. (1989). The speaker as listener. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 353–360.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Lowe, C. F., Home, P. J., Harris, D. S., & Rändle, V. R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: Vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Nuzzolo-Gomez, R. & Greer, R. D. (2004). Emergence of Untaught Mands or Tacts with Novel Adjective-Object Pairs as a Function of Instructional History. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 30–47Google Scholar
  22. Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules. New York: Perennial.Google Scholar
  23. Skinner, B. F. (1989). The behavior of the listener. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies and instructional control (85–96). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Skinner, B.F. (1957,1992). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • R. Douglas Greer
    • 1
    • 2
  • Lauren Stolfi
    • 1
    • 2
  • Mapy Chavez-Brown
    • 1
    • 2
  • Celestina Rivera-Valdes
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Teachers CollegeNew YorkUSA
  2. 2.The Graduate School of Arts and SciencesColumbia UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations