Advertisement

The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 20, Issue 1, pp 77–97 | Cite as

Meaning: A Verbal Behavior Account

  • Barry Lowenkron
Article

Abstract

Although the verbal operants that comprise Skinner’s account of verbal behavior provide a seemingly complete description of the behavior of the speaker with respect to what is ordinarily called the expression of meanings, it may be shown that the account is intrinsically deficient in describing the receptive behavior of listeners with regard to their comprehension of the meanings of novel words, sentences and propositions. In response to this perceived deficiency, the notion of joint control is presented here. Joint control occurs when a verbal-operant topography, currently evoked by one stimulus, is additionally (i.e., jointly) evoked by a second stimulus. This event of joint stimulus control then sets the occasion for a response. This simple mechanism is shown here to have exceedingly broad explanatory properties: providing a coherent and rigorously behavioral account of various aspects of language ranging from meaning, reference and comprehension, to the development of abstraction in children’s speech.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183–200.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. & Garrett, M. F. (1974). The Psychology of Language. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
  3. Gamer, W.R. (1962). Uncertainty and structure as psychological constructs. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  4. Home, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  6. Kendler, H., & Kendler, T. (1962). Vertical and horizontal processes in problem solving. Psychological Review, 69, 1–16.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Lowenkron, B. (1984). Coding responses and the generalization of matching-to-sample in children. Journal ofthe Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Lowenkron, B. (1988). Generalization of delayed identity matching in retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 163–172.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Lowenkron, B. (1989). Instructional control of generalized relational matching to sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 293–309.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Lowenkron, B. (1991). Joint control and the generalization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–126.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Lowenkron, B. (1996). Joint control and word- object bidirectionality. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 252–255.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Lowenkron, B. (1997). The role of naming in the development of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, 244–247.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical function of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Lowenkron, B. & Colvin, V. (1992). Joint control and generalized nonidentity matching: Saying when something is not. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 1–10.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Lowenkron, B. & Colvin, V. (1995). Generalized instructional control and the production of broadly applicable relation responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 12, 13–29.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Michael, J. (1996). Separate repertoires or naming? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 296–298.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Mower, O. H. (1960). Learning theory and the symbolic process. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Nelson, K. E. & Bonvillian, J. D. (1973), Concepts and words in the 2-year old: Acquiring of concept names under controlled conditions. Cognition, 2, 435–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Noble, C. E. (1952). An Analysis of meaning. Psychological Review, 59, 421–430CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Osgood, C. E. (1952). The nature and measure of meaning. Psychological Bulletin, 49, 197–237.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: Perennial Classics.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  23. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Deptartment of PsychologyCalifornia State University—Los AngelesLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations