Skip to main content
Log in

Meaning: A Verbal Behavior Account

  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although the verbal operants that comprise Skinner’s account of verbal behavior provide a seemingly complete description of the behavior of the speaker with respect to what is ordinarily called the expression of meanings, it may be shown that the account is intrinsically deficient in describing the receptive behavior of listeners with regard to their comprehension of the meanings of novel words, sentences and propositions. In response to this perceived deficiency, the notion of joint control is presented here. Joint control occurs when a verbal-operant topography, currently evoked by one stimulus, is additionally (i.e., jointly) evoked by a second stimulus. This event of joint stimulus control then sets the occasion for a response. This simple mechanism is shown here to have exceedingly broad explanatory properties: providing a coherent and rigorously behavioral account of various aspects of language ranging from meaning, reference and comprehension, to the development of abstraction in children’s speech.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. & Garrett, M. F. (1974). The Psychology of Language. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamer, W.R. (1962). Uncertainty and structure as psychological constructs. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Home, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, H., & Kendler, T. (1962). Vertical and horizontal processes in problem solving. Psychological Review, 69, 1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1984). Coding responses and the generalization of matching-to-sample in children. Journal ofthe Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1988). Generalization of delayed identity matching in retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 163–172.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1989). Instructional control of generalized relational matching to sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 293–309.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1991). Joint control and the generalization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–126.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1996). Joint control and word- object bidirectionality. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 252–255.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1997). The role of naming in the development of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, 244–247.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical function of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. & Colvin, V. (1992). Joint control and generalized nonidentity matching: Saying when something is not. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 1–10.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. & Colvin, V. (1995). Generalized instructional control and the production of broadly applicable relation responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 12, 13–29.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1996). Separate repertoires or naming? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 296–298.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mower, O. H. (1960). Learning theory and the symbolic process. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. E. & Bonvillian, J. D. (1973), Concepts and words in the 2-year old: Acquiring of concept names under controlled conditions. Cognition, 2, 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, C. E. (1952). An Analysis of meaning. Psychological Review, 59, 421–430

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1952). The nature and measure of meaning. Psychological Bulletin, 49, 197–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: Perennial Classics.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barry Lowenkron.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lowenkron, B. Meaning: A Verbal Behavior Account. Analysis Verbal Behav 20, 77–97 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392996

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392996

Navigation