Skip to main content
Log in

The Selectionist Meaning of C. S. Peirce and B. F. Skinner

  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The selectionist meaning of C. S. Peirce and B. F Skinner, which has an empirical existence, is advanced against essentialist meaning, which does not. Against a tradition that advocates essentialist meanings, the development of selectionist meaning is traced from Darwin through Peirce and on to Skinner. The views of Peirce and Skinner on meaning are presented as sharing a compatible conceptual foundation with contrasting but complementary distinctions. Support for selectionist meaning comes from contemporary dictionary construction, pragmatist philosophers, and recent views of scientific verbal behavior. Some implications are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bacon, F. (1960). The new organon and related writings (F. H. Anderson, Ed.). New York: Macmillian. (Original work published 1620)

  • Bain, A. (1868). The senses and the intellect (3rd ed.). London: Longmans, Green.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, N. (1958). The autobiography of Charles Darwin 1809–1882. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behrens, P. J. (1993). Peirce’s psychophysics then and now. In E. C. Moore (Ed.), Charles S. Peirce and the philosophy of science: Papers from the Harvard Sesquicentennial Congress (pp. 309–318). Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berg, D. L. (1993). A guide to the Oxford English dictionary. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanshard, B., & Skinner, B. F (1966–1967). The problem of consciousness—a debate. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 27, 317–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to behaviorism. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadwallader, T. C. (1974). Charles S. Peirce (1839–1914): The first American experimental psychologist. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 10, 291–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, G. N. (1987). Weighing light: The role of metaphor in eighteenth-century optical discourse. In A. E. Benjamin, G. N. Cantor, & J. R. R. Christie (Eds.), The figural and the literal: Problems of language in the history of science and philosophy, 1630–1800 (pp. 124–146). Manchester, England: Manchester University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carnap, R. (1934). The unity of science (M. Black, Trans.). London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassirer, E. (1956). Determinism and indeterminism in modern physics (O. T Benfey, Trans.). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. (Original work published 1936)

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1973). The concept of the operant in the analysis of behavior. Behaviorism, 1, 103–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crapanzano, V. (2000). Serving the word: Literalism in America from the pulpit to the bench. New York: The New Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Alembert, J. (1995). Preliminary discourse to the encyclopedia of Diderot (R. N. Schwab, Trans.). Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1751)

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1958). The origin of species. New York: The New American Library of World Literature. (Original work published 1859)

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1872)

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1971). The descent of man. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Original work published in 1871)

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, F. (Ed.). (1887). The life and letters of Charles Darwin, including an autobiographical chapter (Vols. 1–3). London: John Murray.

  • Day, W. F. (1969). On certain similarities between the Philosophical Investigations of Ludwig Wittgenstein and the operationism of B. F Skinner. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 489–506.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Day, W. F. (1980). The historical antecedents of contemporary behaviorism. In R. W. Rieber & K. Salzinger (Eds.), Psychology: Theoretical-historical perspectives (pp. 203–262). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1991). In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, D. Murdoch, A. Kenny (Trans.), The philosophical writing of Descartes (Vol. 3). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

  • Dewey, J. (1966). Democracy and education. New York: Macmillan. (Original work published 1916)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1977). The influence of Darwinism on philosophy. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: The middle works, 1899–1924: Vol. 4. 1907–1909 (pp. 3–30). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press. (Original work published 1909)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1988). Concerning alleged immediate knowledge of mind. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: The middle works, 1899–1924: Vol. 11. 1918–1919 (pp. 10–17). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press. (Original work published 1918)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1988). In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: The later works, 1925–1953: Vol. 1. 1925: Experience and nature. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press. (Original work published 1925)

  • Dewey, J. (1989). How we think. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), John Dewey: The later works, 1925–1953: Vol. 8. 1933: Essays and how we think (rev. ed., pp. 105–352). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press. (Original work published 1933)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J., & Bentley, A. (1947). Definition. The Journal of Philosophy, 44, 282–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Du Bois-Reymond, E. (1874). The limits of our knowledge of nature. Popular Science Monthly, 5, 7–32. (Original work published 1872)

    Google Scholar 

  • Eco, U. (1997). The search for the perfect language. Oxford, England: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. (1990). Experiment and the making of meaning: Human agency in scientific observation and experiment. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1996). Full house: The spread of excellence from Plato to Darwin. New York: Three Rivers Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hammett, D. (1992). The continental op (S. Marcus, Ed.). New York: Vintage.

  • Hayes, S. C., & Brownstein, A. J. (1986). Mentalism, behavior-behavior relations, and a behavior-analytic view of the purposes of science. The Behavior Analyst, 9, 175–190.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1989). The verbal action of the listener as a basis for rule-governance. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 154–190). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Herschel, J. F. W. (1987). A preliminary discourse on the study of natural philosophy. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1830)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, D. L. (1989). The metaphysics of evolution. Albany: State University of New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huxley, T. H. (1887). On the reception of the Origin of Species. In F. Darwin (Ed.), The life and letters of Charles Darwin (Vol. 2, pp. 179–204). London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1880). Great men, great thoughts, and the environment. The Atlantic Monthly, 46, 441–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1975). Pragmatism and the meaning of truth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1983). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1890)

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, J. (1971). Semantic theory. In D. D. Steinberg & L. A. Jakobovits (Eds.), Semantics: An interdisciplinary reader in philosophy, linguistics and psychology (pp. 297–307). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ketner, K. L., & Kloesel, C. J. W. (Eds.). (1986). Peirce, semeiotic, and pragmatism: Essays by Max H. Fisch. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knowlson, J. (1975). Universal language schemes in England and France 1600–1800. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1996). The structure of scientific revolutions (3rd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (2000). The road since structure (J. Conant & J. Haugland, Eds.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  • Lamal, P. A. (1983). A cogent critique of epistemology leaves radical behaviorism unscathed. Behaviorism, 11, 103–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, S. I. (1989). Dictionaries: The art and craft of lexicography. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laplace, P. S. (1951). A philosophical essay on probabilities (F. W. Truscott & F. L. Emory, Trans.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1814)

    Google Scholar 

  • Leigland, S. (1999). Pragmatism, science and society: A review of Richard Rorty’s Objectivity, Relativism, and Truth: Philosophical Papers Volume 1. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 71, 483–500.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, W. (1963). The letters of Wyndham Lewis (W. K. Rose, Ed.). Norfolk, CT: New Directions.

  • Locke, J. (1975). An essay concerning human understanding. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1689)

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyell, C. (1873). The geological evidences of the antiquity of man (4th ed., rev.). London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1991). One long argument: Charles Darwin and the genesis of modern evolutionary thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, D. L., & Morgan, R. K. (2001). Single-participant research design. American Psychologist, 56, 119–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, E. K. (1988). Contextualism: The world view of behavior analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 46, 289–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moxley, R. A. (1997). Skinner: From essentialist to selectionist meaning. Behavior and Philosophy, 25, 95–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moxley, R. A. (1999). H. G. Wells and B. F. Skinner on the superorganism. The Behavior Analyst, 22, 131–148.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moxley, R. A. (2001a). The modern/postmodern context of Skinner’s selectionist turn in 1945. Behavior and Philosophy, 29, 121–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moxley, R. A. (2001b). Sources of Skinner’s pragmatic selectionism in 1945. The Behavior Analyst 24, 201–212.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, M. (1870). The science of language. Nature, 1, 6–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neurath, O. (1959). Sociology and physicalism. In A. J. Ayer (Ed.), Logical positivism (pp. 282–317). New York: The Free Press. (Original work published 1931–1932)

    Google Scholar 

  • Neurath, O. (1983). Individual sciences, unified science, pseudorationalism. In R. S. Cohen & M. Neurath (Eds.), Otto Neurath: Philosophical papers 1913–1946 (pp. 132–138). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: D. Reidel. (Original work published 1936)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Neurath, O. (1983). Physicalism: The philosophy of the Viennese Circle. In R. Cohen & M. Neurath (Eds.), Otto Neurath: Philosophical papers 1913–1946 (pp. 48–51). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: D. Reidel. (Original work published 1931)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1931–1958). In C. Hartshorne, P. Weiss, & A. Burks (Eds.), Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (Vols. 1–8). Cambridge, MA: Belknap.

  • Peirce, C. S. (1982). Writings of Charles S. Peirce: A chronological edition: Vol. 1 (1857–1866). (M. H. Fisch, C. J. W. Kloesel, E. C. Moore, D. D. Roberts, & L. A. Ziegler, Eds.). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

  • Peirce, C. S. (1985). Hume’s arguments against miracles, and the idea of natural law. In C. Eisele (Ed.), Historical perspectives on Peirce’s logic of science (pp. 904–912). New York: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1986). Logic. Chapter 1. Thinking as cerebration. In Writings of Charles S. Peirce: A chronological edition: Vol. 4. 1879–1884 (pp. 45–46). Bloomington: Indiana University. (Original manuscript written 1880)

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1992). How to make our ideas clear. In N. Houser & C. Kloesel (Eds.), The essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings: Vol. 1 (1867–1893) (pp. 124–141). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. (Original work published 1878)

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1998). The first rule of logic. In N. Houser, J. R. Eller, A. C. Lewis, A. D. Tienne, C. L. Clark, & D. B. Davis (Eds.), The essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings: Vol. 2 (1893–1913) (pp. 42–56). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. (Original paper delivered 1898)

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1998). Pragmatism. In N. Houser, J. R. Eller, A. C. Lewis, A. D. Tienne, C. L. Clark, & D. B. Davis (Eds.), The essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings: Vol. 2 (1893–1913) (pp. 398–433). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. (Original manuscript written 1907)

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1998). Chance, love, and logic (M. Cohen, Ed.). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. (Original work published 1923)

  • Quine, W. V. (1981). The pragmatist’s place in empiricism. In R. J. Mulvaney & P. M. Zeltner (Eds.), Pragmatism: Its sources and prospects (pp. 21–39). Columbia: University of South Carolina Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, R. (1950). Definition. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rorty, R. (1992). Twenty-five years after. In R. Rorty (Ed.), The linguistic turn: Essays in philosophical method (pp. 371–374). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, P. (2000). Logic and the art of memory: The quest for a universal language. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1926). Review of The Meaning of Meaning. Dial, 81, 114–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1981). Introduction. Tractatus logico-philosophicus (pp. 7–23) by L. Wittgenstein (C. K. Ogden, Trans.). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1922)

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiappa, E. (1993). Arguing about definitions. Argumentation, 7, 403–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, F. C. S. (1927). Symposium: The problem of meaning. Aristotelian Society: Supplementary Volume VII: Mind, Objectivity and Fact, 98–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, F. C. S. (1930). Logic for use. New York: Harcourt, Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, K. (2001). Harriet who? Traverse: Northern Michigan’s Magazine, 21(4), 62–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, S. M. (1997). Back to our philosophical roots: A journal review of Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society. The Behavior Analyst, 20, 17–23.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, G. (1973). Habit and meaning in Peirce’s pragmatism. Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society, 9, 24–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidgwick, A. (1892). Distinction and the criticism of beliefs. London: Longmans, Green.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidgwick, A. (1910). The application of logic. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, J. A., & Weiner, E. S. C. (1989). The Oxford English dictionary (2nd ed.). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. The Psychological Review, 52, 270–277, 291–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). Verbal behavior: William James lectures Harvard University 1948. Available from the B. F Skinner Foundation Morgantown Office, Room 507A Allen Hall, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV 26507-6122. (e-mail: info@bfskinner.org.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1966). The behavior of organisms. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. (Original work published 1938)

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1972). Freedom and the control of men. In his Cumulative Record (3rd ed., pp. 3–24). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1973). Answers for my critics. In H. Wheeler (Ed.), Beyond the punitive society (pp. 256–266). San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1976). Particulars of my life. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1979). Interview with B. F. Skinner (J. E. Morrow, Ed.). Behaviorists for Social Action Journal, 2, 47–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1980). Notebooks. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1984). The shaping of a behaviorist: Part two of an autobiography. Washington Square: New York University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1986). The evolution of verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 45, 115–122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1987). Some thoughts about the future. In his Upon further reflection (pp. 193–206). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1988). In A. C. Catania & S. Harnad (Eds.), The selection of behavior: The operant behaviorism of B. F. Skinner: Comments and consequences. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1984)

  • Skinner, B. F. (1989a). The behavior of organisms at 50. In his Recent issues in the analysis of behavior (pp. 121–135). Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1989b). The listener. In his Recent issues in the analysis of behavior (pp. 35–47). Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1989c). Whatever happened to psychology as the science of behavior? In his Recent issues in the analysis of behavior (pp. 59–71). Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1997). Appendix. In B. F. Skinner & M. E. Vaughan, Enjoy old age (pp. 155–157). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter, M. M. (1982). Universal languages and scientific taxonomy in the seventeenth century. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprat, T. (1958). The history of the Royal Society (J. I. Cope & H. W. Jones, Eds.). Saint Louis, MO: Washington University. (Original work published 1667)

  • Staddon, J. (2001). The new behaviorism: Mind, mechanism, and society. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, C. (1994). “Nullus in verba” and “nihil in verbis”: Public understanding of the role of language in science. British Journal for the History of Science, 27, 55–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swift, J. (1967). Gulliver’s travels (P. Dixon & J. Chalker, Eds.). London: Penguin. (Original work published 1726)

  • Taub, L. (1993). Evolutionary ideas and “empirical” methods: The analogy between language and species in works by Lyell and Schleicher. British Journal for the History of Science, 26 (Part 2 No. 89), 171–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer, H. S. (1981). Meaning and action: A critical history of pragmatism. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, S. (1972). Human understanding (Vol. 1). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  • Toulmin, S. (1997). Descartes, Descartes … In W. Kayzer (Ed.), A glorious accident (pp. 177–210). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, S. (2001). Return to reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, H. G. (1904). Scepticism of the instrument. Mind, 13, 379–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, H. G. (1908). First and last things. London: G. P. Putnam’s Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willinsky, J. (1994). Empire of words: The reign of the OED. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Winchester, S. (1998). The professor and the madman: A tale of murder, insanity, and the making of the Oxford English dictionary. New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations (3rd ed.). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1960). The blue and brown books: Preliminary studies for the “Philosophical Investigations” (2nd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1969). On certainty (G. E. M. Anscombe & G. H. von Wright, Eds.). New York: Harper & Row.

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1981). Tractatus logicophilosophicus (C. K. Ogden, Trans.). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1922)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1982). Last writings on the philosophy of psychology: Vol. 1. Preliminary studies for part II of Philosophical Investigations (G. H. von Wright & H. Nyman, Eds.). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

  • Zuriff, G. E. (1980). Radical behaviorist epistemology. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 337–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roy A. Moxley.

Additional information

I thank Julie S. Vargas and the B. F. Skinner Foundation for providing a copy of the manuscript of Skinner’s William James lectures. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the First International Association for Behavior Analysis Conference in Venice, 2001.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Moxley, R.A. The Selectionist Meaning of C. S. Peirce and B. F. Skinner. Analysis Verbal Behav 18, 71–91 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392972

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392972

Navigation