Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of a Stimulus-Stimulus Pairing Procedure on Conditioning Vocal Sounds as Reinforcers

  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments were conducted to investigate the effects of stimulus-stimulus pairing on conditioning vocal sounds as reinforcers. Four preschoolers with severe language and communication delays participated. In Experiment 1, an attempt was made to condition vocal sounds as a reinforcer by pairing a specific vocal sound with a reinforcing event (e.g., physical interaction). Results indicated that presentation of a stimulus-stimulus pairing was effective in conditioning the target vocal sounds as reinforcers, which increased the occurrence of vocalizations of those sounds by the participants. Experiment 2 compared the effects of the pairing procedure with those of echoic training. The pairing procedure was identical to that in Experiment 1. In the echoic training condition, the experimenter produced the target vocal sound and gave the participant an opportunity to echo. The same reinforcing stimulus (e.g., physical interaction) was provided contingent upon the occurrence of the target vocal sound emitted by the participant. Results showed that the pairing procedure was more effective than the echoic training. Findings from this study suggest that for these participants, who had no vocal imitation skills, the stimulus-stimulus pairing was an effective procedure for conditioning vocal sounds as reinforcers and increased the probability of occurrence of the vocalization without a direct reinforcement contingency.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1965). Child development: Vol. 2. Universal stage of infancy. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 11–53). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. G. (1992). Language and the continuity of species. In S. C. Hayes & L. J. Hayes (Eds.), Understanding verbal relations (pp. 197–209). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kravitz, H., & Boehm, J. (1971). Rhythmic habit patterns in infancy: Their sequences, age of onset, and frequency. Child Development, 42, 399–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I., Vami, J. W., Koegel, R. L., & Lorsch, N. (1977). Some observations on the nonextinguishability of children’s speech. Child Development, 48, 1121–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E. L. (1983). A behavioral analysis of controversial topics in first language acquisition: Reinforcements, corrections, modeling, input frequencies, and the three-term contingency. Journal of Psycholin- guistic Research, 12, 129–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E. L. (1990). Three-term contingency patterns in mother-child verbal interactions during first-language acquisition. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 293–305.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1954). The psychologist looks at language. American Psychology, 9, 660–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakazima, S. (1962). A comparative study of the speech developments of Japanese and American English in childhood. Study of Phonology, 2, 27–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R., Michael, J., & Sundberg, M. L. (1996). Automatic reinforcement and automatic punishment in infant vocal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 13, 39–48.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, M. L., Michael, J., Partington, J. W., & Sundberg, C. A. (1996). The role of automatic reinforcement in early language acquisition. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 13, 21–37.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behavior, 27, 699–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1981). Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology, 17, 237–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, M. E., & Michael, J. L. (1982). Automatic reinforcement: An important but ignored concept. Behaviorism, 10, 217–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, S. (1998). Effects of an adult’s vocal sound paired with a reinforcing event on the subsequent acquisition of mand functions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to So-Young Yoon.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yoon, SY., Bennett, G.M. Effects of a Stimulus-Stimulus Pairing Procedure on Conditioning Vocal Sounds as Reinforcers. Analysis Verbal Behav 17, 75–88 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392957

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392957

Navigation