Advertisement

The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 15, Issue 1, pp 75–91 | Cite as

Memory as Behavior: The Importance of Acquisition and Remembering Strategies

Article

Abstract

The study of memory has traditionally been the province of cognitive psychology, which has postulated different memory systems that store memory traces to explain remembering. Behavioral psychologists have been unsuccessful at empirically identifying the behavior that occurs during remembering because so much of it occurs rapidly and covertly. In addition, behavior analysts have generally been disinterested in studying transient phenomena such as memory. As a result, the cognitive interpretation has been the only one that has made and tested useful predictions. Recent experimental evidence acquired while having participants “think aloud” suggests that a behavioral approach to memory may provide a superior account of memory performance and allow applied scientists to observe and modify memory-related behavior with well-known applied behavior-analytic techniques. We review evidence supporting and extending the interpretation of memory provided by Palmer (1991), who described memory in terms of precurrent behavior that occurs at the time of acquisition in preparation for problem solving that occurs at the time of remembering.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson, M. C., & Neely, J. H. (1996). Interference and inhibition in memory retrieval. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory (pp. 237–313). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ashcraft, M. H. (1989). Human memory and cognition. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
  3. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In W. K. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Augustine. (1977). Confessions (F. J. Sheed, Trans.). In S. M. Cahn (Ed.), Classics of western philosophy (pp. 345–361). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing Company. (Original work published 398)Google Scholar
  5. Austin, J., & Delaney, P. F. (1998). Protocol analysis as a tool for behavior analysis. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 15, 41–56.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 123–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bellezza, F. S. (1981). Mnemonic devices: Classification, characteristics, and criteria. Review of Educational Research, 51, 247–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bellezza, F. S. (1996). Mnemonic methods for storage and retrieval. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory (pp. 345–380). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (1996). Preface. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory (pp. xix–xxii). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bourgeois, M. S. (1992). Evaluating memory wallets in conversations with persons with dementia. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 35, 1344–1357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bower, G. H., & Clark, M. C. (1969). Narrative stories as mediators for serial learning. Psychonomic Science, 14, 181–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Branch, M. N. (1977). On the role of “memory” in the analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 28, 171–179.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Catania, A. C. (1992). Learning (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  15. Charness, N. (1976). Memory for chess positions: Resistance to interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 641–653.Google Scholar
  16. Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1981). Skilled memory. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 141–189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  17. Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 1–58). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Crowder, R. G. (1989). Modularity and dissociations in memory systems. In H. L. Roediger, III & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness (pp. 271–294). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. de Groot, A. (1978). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton. (Original work published 1946)Google Scholar
  23. Delaney, P. F, Reder, L. M., Ritter, F. E., & Staszewski, J. J. (1998). The strategy-specific nature of improvement: The power law applies by strategy within task. Psychological Science, 9, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Donahoe, J. W., & Palmer, D. C. (1994). Learning and complex behavior. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
  25. Ebbinghaus, H. (1964). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussenius, Trans.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1885)Google Scholar
  26. Ericsson, K. A. (1985). Memory skill. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 188–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725–747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 208, 1181–1182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ericsson, K. A., & Delaney, P. F. (in press). Long-term working memory as an alternative to capacity models of working memory in everyday skilled performance. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Ericsson, K. A., Delaney, P. F., Weaver, G. E., & Mahadevan, R. S. (1996, November). The memorist Rajan’s memory for digits and other symbols: Extensive practice and/or great talent? Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society, 1, 68.Google Scholar
  31. Ericsson, K. A., & Harris, M. S. (1990). Expert chess memory without chess knowledge: A training study. Poster presented at the 31st annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans.Google Scholar
  32. Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence for maximal adaptations to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  35. Frey, P. W., & Adesman, P. (1976). Recall memory for visually presented chess positions. Memory and Cognition, 4, 541–547.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Recall of rapidly presented random chess positions is a function of skill. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 159–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hayes, S. C. (1986). The case of the silent dog - verbal reports and the analysis of rules: A review of Ericsson and Simon’s Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 351–363.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Hayes, S. C., White, D., & Bissett, R. T. (1998). Protocol analysis and the “silent dog” method of analyzing the impact of self-generated rules. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 15, 57–63.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  40. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kearins, J. M. (1981). Visual spatial memory in Australian aboriginal children of desert regions. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 434–460.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. M. (1950). Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  43. Kiess, H. O., & Montague, W. E. (1965). Natural language mediators in paired associates learning. Psychonomic Science, 3, 549–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kintsch, W., & Buschke, H. (1969). Homophones and synonyms in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 403–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Klahr, D., Chase, W. G., & Lovelace, E. A. (1983). Structure and process in alphabetic retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 9, 462–477.Google Scholar
  46. Lindsley, O. R. (1996). Is fluency free-operant response-response chaining? The Behavior Analyst, 19, 211–224.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits of our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Montague, W. E. (1972). Elaborative strategies in verbal learning and memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 6, pp. 225–302). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Müller, G. E., & Schumann, F. (1894). Experimentelle beitrage zur Untersuchung des gedächtnisses [Ex-perimental contributions to the study of memory]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 6, 81–190.Google Scholar
  50. Nairne, J. S. (1996). Short-term /working memory. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Memory (pp. 101–126). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  52. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Paivio, A. (1979). Imagery and verbal processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  54. Paivio, A., & Okovita, H. W. (1971). Word imagery modalities and associative learning in blind and sighted subjects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 506–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Palmer, D. C. (1991). A behavioral interpretation of memory. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior: The first international institute on verbal relations (pp. 261–279). Reno, NV: Context Press.Google Scholar
  56. Parsons, J. A., Taylor, D. C., & Joyce, T. M. (1981). Precurrent self-prompting operants in children: “Remembering.” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 253–266.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Pashler, H. E. (in press). The psychology of attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  58. Potter, B., Huber, S., & Michael, J. (1997). The role of mediating verbal behavior in selection-based responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 14, 41–56.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. Reed, H. B. (1918). Associative aids I: Their relation to learning, retention, and other associations. Psychological Review, 25, 128–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Ritter, F. E., & Bibby, P. G. (1997). Modeling how and why learning happens in a diagrammatic reasoning task. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  61. Saariluoma, P. (1985). Chess players’ intake of task-relevant cues. Memory and Cognition, 13, 385–391.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Saariluoma, P. (1989). Chess players’ recall of auditorily presented chess positions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 1, 309–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Siegler, R. S. (1987). The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children’s addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 115, 250–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Siegler, R. S. (1991). Children’s thinking. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  65. Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
  67. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  69. Skinner, B. F. (1977). Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism, 5, 1–10.Google Scholar
  70. Staszewski, J. J. (1988a). The psychological reality of retrieval structures: An investigation of expert knowledge. Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 2126B.Google Scholar
  71. Staszewski, J. J. (1988b). Skilled memory and expert mental calculation. In M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. J. Fair (Eds.), The nature of expertise (pp. 71–128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  72. Walker, N., & Catrambone, R. (1993). Aggregation bias and the use of regression in evaluating models of human performance. Human Factors, 35, 397–411.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Wallander, R. J., & Elliott, R. G. (1997, May). Behavioral methods for examining cognitive research issues. In J. Austin (Chair), Human memory and remembering behavior. Symposium conducted at the 23rd annual convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Chicago.Google Scholar
  74. Watkins, M. J. (1990). Mediationism and the obfusca-tion of memory. American Psychologist, 45, 328–335.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Whimbey, A. (1995). Analytical reading and reasoning (2nd ed.). Stamford, CT: Innovative Sciences.Google Scholar
  76. Wixted, J. T. (1989). The vocabulary of remembering: A review of Kendrick, Rilling, and Denny’s Theories of Animal Memory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 441–450.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  77. Wulfert, E., Dougher, M. J., & Greenway, D. E. (1991). Protocol analysis of the correspondence of verbal behavior and equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 489–504.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  78. Yates, F. A. (1966). The art of memory. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyFlorida State UniversityTallahasseeUSA
  2. 2.Western Michigan UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations