Skip to main content
Log in

Intraverbal Stimulus-Response Reversibility: Fluency, Familiarity Effects, and Implications for Stimulus Equivalence

  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

English-speaking subjects with little knowledge of the French language used a computerized flashcard program, Think Fast, to learn 16 English-French word pairs (intraverbals) by typing one word of the pair when presented with the other word as a textual stimulus. In Phase 1, half of the intraverbals were taught from French to English (FE-1) and half from English to French (EF-1). Then, in Phase 2, training continued with the stimulus and response items of each intraverbal reversed, i.e., cards previously in the FE-1 condition were trained from English to French (EF-2) and cards previously in the EF-1 condition were trained from French to English (FE-2). Feedback was provided throughout the experiment. Reversing the stimulus and response items in Phase 2 significantly reduced rate correct and accuracy scores for eight of the nine subjects. In Experiments 1 and 2, this effect was more pronounced for cards in the EF-2 condition; in Experiment 3, when the criterion for a “correct” response was more lenient, there was no consistent difference between cards in the EF-2 and FE-2 conditions. Symmetry, as indicated by accuracy scores on the first trial in Phase 2, was generally poor: eight of the nine subjects averaged only 29% correct when asked to respond to the reversed relations for the first time. We relate our paradigm and results to recent developments in fluency, verbal behavior, and stimulus equivalence, and provide directions for future research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Binder, C. (1993). Behavioral fluency: A new paradigm. Educational Technology, 33, 8–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, C. (1996). Behavioral fluency: Evolution of a new paradigm. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 163–197.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, C., Haughton, E., & Van Eyk, D. (1990). Precision teaching attention span. Teaching Exceptional Children, 22, 24–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, C., & Watkins, C. L. (1990). Precision teaching and direct instruction: Measurably superior instructional technology in schools. Performance Improvement Quarterly, 3, 74–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1992). Learning (3rd ed.) Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dale, E., & O’Rourke, J. (1976). The living world vocabulary: The words we know: A national vocabulary inventory. Chicago: Field Enterprises Educational Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dougher, M. J., Augustson, E., Markam, M. R., Greenway, D. E., Wulfert, E. (1994). The transfer of respondent eliciting and extinction functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 331–351.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dougherty, K. M., & Johnson, J. M. (1996). Overlearning, fluency, and automaticity. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 289–292.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, G. A., & Chase, P. N. (1991). The relationship between stimulus equivalence and verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 107–119.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Home, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. R., & Layng, T. V. J. (1992). Breaking the structuralist barrier: Literacy and numeracy with fluency. American Psychologist, 47, 1475–1490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. R., & Layng, T. V. J. (1996). On terms and procedures: Fluency. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 281–288.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, S. D., & Critchfield, T. S. (1996). Verbal self-reports of emergent relations in a stimulus equivalence procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 355–374.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, O. R. (1992). Precision teaching: Discoveries and effects. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 51–57.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsely, O.R. (1996a). The four free-operant freedoms. The Behavior Analyst, 19, 199–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, O.R. (1996b). Is fluency free-operant response-response chaining? The Behavior Analyst, 29, 211–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., & Beasty, A. (1987). Language and the emergence of equivalence relations: A developmen-tal study. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 40, A49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1991). Joint control and the general-ization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–126.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, D. C., & Cuvo, A. J. (1995). Stimulus equiva-lence instruction of fraction-decimal relations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 115–126.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mandell, C., & Sheen, V. (1994). Equivalence class formation as a function of the pronounceability of the sample stimulus. Behavioural Process, 32, 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1985). Two kinds of verbal behavior plus a possible third. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 3, 2–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, S. E. (1972). In search of associative symmetry. In C. P. Duncan, L. Sechrest, & A W. Melton (Eds.), Human memory: Festschrift for Benton J. Underwood (pp. 133–153). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olander, C. P., Collins, D. L., McArthur, B. L., Watts, W. O., & McDade, C. E. (1986). Retention among college students: A comparison of traditional versus precision teaching. Journal of Precision Teaching, 4, 80–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, J. A. (1989). Think Fast. [Computer program]. Victoria, BC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, J. A., Taylor, D. C., & Joyce, T. M. (1981). Precurrent self-prompting operants in children: “Remembering.” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 253–266.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Poison, D. A. D. (1995). Fostering multiple repertoires in undergraduate behavior analysis students. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 293–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poison, D. A. D., & Parsons, J. A. (1994). Precurrent contingencies: Behavior reinforced by altering rein-forcement probability for other behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 427–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poison, D. A. D., Wong, W. K., & Parsons, J. A., & Grabavac, D. M. (1991, May). Using Think Fast to examine variables affecting the efficacy of flash card instruction. Poster presented at the Association for Behavior Analysis 17th Annual Convention, Atlanta, GA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. R., & Green, G. (1996). Naming and equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 312–314.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shafer, E. (1993). Teaching topography-based and selection-based verbal behavior to developmentally disabled individuals: Some considerations. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 11, 117–133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, T. J., & Chase, P. N. (1996). Speed analyses of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 643–659.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, C. T., & Sundberg, M. L. (1990). Comparing topography-based verbal behavior with stimulus selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 31–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wraikat, R., Sundberg, C. T., Michael, J. (1991). Topography-based and selection-based verbal behavior: A further comparison. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 1–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yuen, S., Poison, D. A. D., & Parsons, J. A. (1992, May). A study comparing the effectiveness of key-words and answers using Think Fast. In G. E. Yaber (Chair), Think Fast: An authoring/delivery system for behavioral research and training. Symposium conducted at the Association for Behavior Analysis 18th Annual Convention, San Francisco, CA.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David A. D. Polson.

Additional information

Portions of the data were presented at the 1992 convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis in San Francisco, CA.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Polson, D.A.D., Grabavac, D.M. & Parsons, J.A. Intraverbal Stimulus-Response Reversibility: Fluency, Familiarity Effects, and Implications for Stimulus Equivalence. Analysis Verbal Behav 14, 19–40 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392914

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392914

Navigation