The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 8, Issue 1, pp 127–140 | Cite as

On the relationship between speech and writing with implications for behavioral approaches to teaching literacy

  • Roy A. Moxley
Article
  • 2 Downloads

Abstract

Two theories of the relationship between speech and writing are examined. One theory holds that writing is restricted to a one-way relationship with speech—a unidirectional influence from speech to writing. In this theory, writing is derived from speech and is simply a representation of speech. The other theory holds that additional, multidirectional influences are involved in the development of writing. The unidirectional theory focuses on correspondences between speech and writing while the multidirectional theory directs attention to the differences as well as the similarities between speech and writing. These theories have distinctive pedagogical implications. Although early behaviorism may be seen to have offered some support for the unidirectional theory, modern behavior analysis should be seen to support the multidirectional theory.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, R. C., Hiebart, E. F., Scott, J. A., & Wilkinson, I. A. (1985). Emerging literacy. In Becoming a nation of readers (pp. 21–58). Washington, DC: The National Institute of Education.Google Scholar
  3. Aristotle (1938). On interpretation (H. P. Cook, trans.). In The Organon (Vol. 1, pp. 115–179). London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  4. Bailey, J. S. (1987). On having an impact. The ABA Newsletter, 10 (2).Google Scholar
  5. Becker, W. (1977). Teaching reading and language to the disadvantaged—What we have learned from field research. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 518–543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bereiter, C. (1986). Does direct instruction cause delinquency? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 1, 289–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  8. Bloomfield, L. (1942). Linguistics and reading. Elementary English Review, 19, 125–130, 183–186.Google Scholar
  9. Bloomfield, L., & Barnhart, C. (1961). Let’s read. Detroit: Wayne State U. Press.Google Scholar
  10. Carbo, M. (1988). Debunking the great phonics myth. Phi Delta Kappan, 70, 226–240.Google Scholar
  11. Chall, J. S. (1989). Learning to read: The great debate 20 years later—A response to ‘debunking the great phonics myth.’ Phi Delta Kappan, 70, 521–538.Google Scholar
  12. Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of B. F. Skinner, Verbal Behavior (1957). Language, 35, 26–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
  14. Clanchy, M. T. (1979). From memory to written record. Cambridge, MA: Harvard U. Press.Google Scholar
  15. Cudd, E. T., & Roberts, L. L. (1986). Using story frames to develop reading comprehension in a 1st grade classroom, The Reading Teacher, 40, pp. 74–79.Google Scholar
  16. de Saussure, F. (1966). Course in general linguistics, (W. Baskin, Trans.). New York: McGraw-Hill. (Original work published 1915).Google Scholar
  17. Durkin, D. (1966). Children who read early. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
  18. Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1986). The influence of spelling on speech. In D. Yaden and S. Templeton (Eds.), Metalinguistic awareness and beginning literacy (pp. 101–114). Exeter, NH: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  19. Eisenstein, E. L. (1983). The printing revolution in early modern Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge U. Press.Google Scholar
  20. Engelmann, S., Haddox, P., & Bruner, E. (1983). Teach your child to read in 100 easy lessons. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
  21. Esper, E. A. (1968). Mentalism and objectivism in linguistics: The sources of Leonard Bloomfield’s psychology of language. New York: American Elsevier.Google Scholar
  22. Friberg, J. (1984). Numbers and measures in the earliest written records. Scientific American, 250 (2), 110–114, 116–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gelb, I. J. (1963). A study of writing (rev. ed). Chicago: U. of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  24. Goldiamond, I. (1977). Literary behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 527–529.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Goody, J., & Watt, I. (1968). The consequences of literacy. In J. Goody (Ed.), Literacy in traditional societies (pp. 27–68). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Graves, D. H. (1985). All children can write. Learning Disabilities Focus, 1, 36–43.Google Scholar
  27. Halliday, M. A. K. (1987). Spoken and written modes of meaning. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 55–82). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  28. Harris, R. (1986). The origin of writing. La Salle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
  29. Havelock, E. A. (1986). The muse learns to write. New Haven, CN: Yale U. Press.Google Scholar
  30. Hewes, G. W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origin of speech. Current Anthropology, 14, 5–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Holland, J. G. (1979). Analysis of behavior in reading instruction. In L. R. Resnick & P. A. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 1, pp. 261–277). Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  32. Householder, F. W. (1971). The primacy of writing. In his Linguistic speculations (pp. 244–264). London: Cambridge U. Press.Google Scholar
  33. Hummel, J. W. (1985). Courseware Revisited: Cloze plus: A context analysis program. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 18, 364–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hymes, D., & Fought, J. (1981). American structuralism. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  35. Ifrah, G. (1987). From one to zero: A universal history of numbers. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
  36. Jasim, S. A., & Oates, J. (1986). Early tokens and tablets in Mesopotamia. World Archaeology, 17, 348–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Johnston, V. M. (1985). Introducing the microcomputer into English. III. An evaluation of TRAY as a program using problem-solving as a strategy for developing reading skills. British Journal of Educational Technology, 16 (3), 208–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Julia, P. (1983). Explanatory models in linguistics: A behavioral perspective. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kantor, J. R. (1936). An objective psychology of grammar. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Publications, Science Series No. 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kantor, J. R. (1977). Psychological linguistics. Chicago: Principia Press.Google Scholar
  41. Kolers, P. A. (1985). Phonology in reading. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning (pp. 404–411). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Lehr, F. (1987). ERIC/RCS: Story Grammar. The Reading Teacher, 40, 550–552.Google Scholar
  43. LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1985). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. In H. Singer & R. B. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 689–718). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
  44. Moffett, J., & Wagner, B. J. (1983). Student-centered language arts and reading, k-13 (3rd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  45. Moran, M. R. (1987). Epilogue. Topics in language disorders, 7, 86–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Morrow, L. M., & Weinstein, C. S. (1982). Increasing children’s use of literature through program and physical design changes. The Elementary School Journal, 83, 131–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Moxley, R. A. (1982). Writing and reading in early childhood: A functional approach. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Technology.Google Scholar
  48. Moxley, R. A. (1986). A functional analysis of reading. In P. N. Chase & L. J. Parrott (Eds.), Psychological aspects of language (pp. 209–232). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
  49. Moxley, R. A. (1990). A review of Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Educational Technology, 30, (10), 56–57.Google Scholar
  50. Moxley, R. A., & Joyce, B. G. (1990). Early visual spelling on the microcomputer. Journal of Computing in Childhood Educaton, 1(3), 3–14.Google Scholar
  51. Nystrand, M. (1987). The role of context in communication. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 197–214). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  52. Olson, D. R., Torrance, N., & Hildyard, A. (Eds.). (1985). Literacy, language, and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge U. Press.Google Scholar
  53. Ong, W. J. (1982). Orality and literacy. New York: Methuen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Otto, J. (1982). The new debate in reading. The Reading Teacher, 36, 14–18.Google Scholar
  55. Peirce, C. S. (1960–1961). Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (C. Hartshorne and P. Weiss [Eds.] Vols. 1–4). Cambridge, MA: Belknap.Google Scholar
  56. Pike, K. L. (1947). Phonemics: A technique for reducing languages to writing. University of Michigan Publications, Linguistics 3. Ann Arbor. MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  57. Powell, R. P., & Still, A. W. (1979). Behaviorism and the psychology of language. Behaviorism, 7, 71–89.Google Scholar
  58. Price, E. H. (1976). How thirty-seven gifted children learned to read. The Reading Teacher, 30, 44–48.Google Scholar
  59. Sapir, E. (1921). Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
  60. Schmandt-Besserat, D. (1978). The earliest precursor of writing. Scientific American, 238 (6), 50–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Schweinhart, L. J., Weikart, D. P., & Larner, M. B. (1986). Consequences of three preschool curriculum models. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 1, 15–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  63. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Skinner, B. F. (1972). Why do we need teaching machines. In his Cumulative record (3rd ed., pp. 171–193) New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  65. Skinner, B. F. (1989). Recent issues in the analysis of behavior. Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar
  66. Smith, F. (1973). Decoding: The great fallacy. In his Psycholinguistics and reading (pp. 70–83). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  67. Smith, F. (1989). Understanding reading (4th ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  68. Staats, A. W. (1968). Learning, language, and cognition. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  69. Steinberg, D. D., & Steinberg, M. T. (1975). Reading before speaking. Visible Language, 9, 197–224.Google Scholar
  70. Stotsky, S. (1983). Research on reading/writing relationships. Language Arts, 60, 627–642.Google Scholar
  71. Stotsky, S. (1987). A comparison of the two theories about development in written language: Implications for pedagogy and research. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 371–395). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  72. Street, B. V. (1984). Literacy in theory and practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  73. Stubbs, M. (1980). Language and literacy. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  74. Stubbs, M. (1986). Educational linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  75. Sulzby, E. (1986). Writing and reading: Signs of oral and written language organization in the young child. In W. H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 50–89). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  76. Taylor, N. E., Blum, I. H., & Logsdon, D. M. (1986). The development of written language awareness: Environmental aspects and program characteristics. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 132–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Torrey, J. W. (1973). Learning to read without a teacher: A case study. In F. Smith (Ed.), Psycholinguistics and reading (pp. 147–157). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  78. Tweney, R. D. (1979). Reflections on the history of behavioral theories of language. Behaviorism, 7, 91–103.Google Scholar
  79. Yates, F. A. (1966). The art of memory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Roy A. Moxley
    • 1
  1. 1.West Virginia UniversityMorgantownUSA

Personalised recommendations