The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 8, Issue 1, pp 19–30 | Cite as

Equivalence class formation in non-hearing impaired children and hearing impaired children

  • Dermot Barnes
  • Paul D. McCullagh
  • Michael Keenan
Article

Abstract

The relationship between verbal behavior and stimulus equivalence was examined using three sets of children differing in chronological age and verbal ability: (1) non-hearing impaired three and four year olds who had verbal skills generally consistent with their chronological ages; (2) partially hearing (severe to profoundly deaf) children who were rated with verbal ages of above 2 years; and (3) partially hearing children (also severely to profoundly deaf) who were rated with verbal ages of below 2 years. All children were taught a series of four conditional discriminations using unfamiliar stimuli. The children were then tested to determine whether classes of equivalent stimuli had formed. Although all the children were able to learn the conditional discriminations equally well and all the verbally-able children (normal and partially hearing) formed equivalence classes, only one of the verbally-impaired children reliably demonstrated stimulus equivalence formation. These results are consistent with the suggestion that stimulus equivalence and human verbal behavior are closely related.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Barnes, D., & Keenan, M. (1989). Positive and negative transer of stimulus functions, and generalization in the control of human schedule performance. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour Group, Cambridge, England.Google Scholar
  2. Devany, J.M., Hayes, S.C., & Nelson, R.O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Dixon, M. (1976). Teaching conceptual classes with receptive label training. Acta Symbolica, 9, 17–35.Google Scholar
  4. Dugdale, N., & Lowe, C.F. (1990). Naming and stimulus equivalence. In D.E. Blackman and H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behaviour analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 115–138). Brighton, U.K.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  5. Fields, L. (1990). The effects of testing protocols on the formation and expansion of equivalence classes. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour Group, York, England.Google Scholar
  6. Hayes, S.C. (1990). A relational control theory of stimulus equivalence. In L.J. Hayes and P.N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior: Proceedings of the First International Institute on Verbal Relations (pp. 19–40). Reno, NV: Context Press.Google Scholar
  7. Hayes, S.C., & Hayes, L.J. (1989). The verbal action of the listener as a basis for rule-governance. In S.C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 153–190). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Makay, H.A., & Sidman, M. (1984). Teaching new behaviors via equivalence relations. In P. Brooks, R. Sperber, & C. McCauley (Eds.), Learning and cognition in the mentally retarded (pp. 493–513). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5–13.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Sidman, M. (1977). Teaching some basic prerequisites to reading. In P. Mittler (Ed.), Research to practice in mental retardation, II. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  11. Sidman, M., Rauzin, R., Lazar, R., Cunningham, S., Tailby, W., & Carrigan, P. (1982). A search for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of rhesus monkeys, baboons and children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 23–44.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Steele, D.L. (1987). The relations of same, opposite, and different in arbitrary matching to sample. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of North Carolina at Greensboro.Google Scholar
  13. Wetherby, B., Karlan, G.R., & Spradlin, J.E. (1983). The development of derived stimulus relations through training in arbitrary matching sequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 69–78.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Dermot Barnes
    • 1
  • Paul D. McCullagh
    • 2
  • Michael Keenan
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Applied PsychologyUniversity College CorkCorkIreland
  2. 2.University of UlsterColeraineNorthern Ireland

Personalised recommendations