The Analysis of Verbal Behavior

, Volume 4, Issue 1, pp 2–9 | Cite as

On the difference between verbal and nonverbal behavior

  • A. Charles Catania
Article

Abstract

Language theorists have often proposed lists of the defining properties of language. This account reviews some of these and offers as an alternative a three-item list consisting of instructional control, equivalence classes, and autoclitic processes.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Catania, A. C. (1973). The psychologies of structure, function, and development. American Psychologist, 28, 434–443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Catania, A. C. (1980). Autoclitic processes and the structure of behavior. Behaviorism, 8, 175–186.Google Scholar
  3. Catania, A. C. (1984). Learning (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  4. Catania, A. C. (in press). Rule-governed behavior and the origins of language. In C. F. Lowe, M. Richelle, D. E. Blackman & C. Bradshaw (Eds.), Behavior analysis and contemporary psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Catania, A. C., & Cerutti, D. (in press). Some nonverbal properties of verbal behavior. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Catania, A. C., Matthews, B. A., & Shimoff, E. (1982). Instructed versus shaped human verbal behavior: Interactions with nonverbal responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 233–248.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Chomsky, N., & Miller, G. A. (1963). Introduction to the formal analysis of natural languages. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology. Volume II. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  8. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
  9. De Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (1974). On this, that, and the other: Nonegocentrism in very young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 438–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fentress, J. C. (1978). Mus musicus, the experimental orchestration of selected movement patterns in mice. In G. M. Burghardt & M. Bekoff (Eds.), The development of behavior: Comparative and evolutionary aspects. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
  11. Gallistel, C. R. (1980). The organization of action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Hockett, C. D. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  13. Hockett, C. D. (1960, March). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203, 88–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hockett, C. D. (1963). The problem of universais in language. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  15. Jaynes, J. (1977). The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  16. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  17. Linden, E. (1981). Apes, men, and language (revised ed.). New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
  18. Lowe, C. F. (1979). Determinants of human operant behaviour. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Reinforcement and the organization of behaviour. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  19. Matthews, B. A., Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., & Sagvolden, T. (1977). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity to ratio and interval contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 453–467.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Miller, G. A. (1965). Some preliminaries to psycholin-guistics. American Psychologist, 20, 15–20.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Osgood, C. E. (1980). Lectures on language performance. New York: Springer Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Paivio, A., & Begg, I. (1981). Psychology of language. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  23. Paniagua, F., & Baer, D. M. (1982). The analysis of correspondence training as a chain reinforceable at any point. Child Development, 53, 786–798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Premack, D. (1970). A functional analysis of language. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, 107–125.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Reddy, M. J. (1979). The conduit metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Sidman, M., Rauzin, R., Lazar, R., & Cunningham, S. (1982). A search for symmetry in the conditional discrimination of Rhesus monkeys, baboons, and children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 23–44.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Skinner, B. F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem solving. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • A. Charles Catania
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology-UMBCUniversity of Maryland Baltimore CountyCatonsvilleUSA

Personalised recommendations