Advertisement

The Behavior Analyst

, Volume 18, Issue 2, pp 225–236 | Cite as

Jacques Loeb, B. F. Skinner, and the Legacy of Prediction and Control

Article

Abstract

The biologist Jacques Loeb is an important figure in the history of behavior analysis. Between 1890 and 1915, Loeb championed an approach to experimental biology that would later exert substantial influence on the work of B. F. Skinner and behavior analysis. This paper examines some of these sources of influence, with a particular emphasis on Loeb’s firm commitment to prediction and control as fundamental goals of an experimental life science, and how these goals were extended and broadened by Skinner. Both Loeb and Skinner adopted a pragmatic approach to science that put practical control of their subject matter above formal theory testing, both based their research programs on analyses of reproducible units involving the intact organism, and both strongly endorsed technological applications of basic laboratory science. For Loeb, but especially for Skinner, control came to mean something more than mere experimental or technological control for its own sake; it became synonomous with scientific understanding. This view follows from (a) the successful working model of science Loeb and Skinner inherited from Ernst Mach, in which science is viewed as human social activity, and effective practical action is taken as the basis of scientific knowledge, and (b) Skinner’s analysis of scientific activity, situated in the world of direct experience and related to practices arranged by scientific verbal communities. From this perspective, prediction and control are human acts that arise from and are maintained by social circumstances in which such acts meet with effective consequences.

Key words

prediction and control historical analysis epistemology Jacques Loeb B. F. Skinner 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Barnes, T. C., & Skinner, B. F. (1930). The progressive increase in the geotropic response of the ant Aphaenogaster. Journal of General Psychology, 4, 102–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Chiesa, M. (1992). Radical behaviorism and scientific frameworks: From mechanistic to relational accounts. American Psychologist, 47, 1287–1299.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Coleman, S. R. (1984). Background and change in B. F. Skinner’s metatheory from 1930 to 1938. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 5, 471–500.Google Scholar
  4. Day, W. F., Jr. (1980). The historical antecedents of contemporary behaviorism. In R. W. Rieber & K. Salzinger (Eds.), Psychology: Theoretical-historical perspectives (pp. 203–262). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Fleming, D. (1964). Introduction. In J. Loeb, The mechanistic conception of life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Goudge, T. A. (1967). Jacques Loeb. The encyclopedia of philosophy. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  7. Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Hayes, S. C. (1978). Theory and technology in behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 1, 25–33.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Herrnstein, R. J. (1972). Nature as nurture: Behaviorism and the instinct doctrine. Behaviorism, 1, 23–52.Google Scholar
  10. Hineline, P. N. (1992). A self-interpretive behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47, 1274–1286.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  12. Kazdin, A. E. (1978). History of behavior modification. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  13. Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  14. Loeb, J. (1900). Comparative physiology of the brain and comparative psychology. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
  15. Loeb, J. (1905). Studies in general physiology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1890)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Loeb, J. (1907). Concerning the theory of tropisms. The Journal of Experimental Zoology, 4, 145–156.Google Scholar
  17. Loeb, J. (1912). The mechanistic conception of life. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Loeb, J. (1916). The organism as a whole. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
  19. Logue, A. W. (1988). The behaviorist’s biologist: A review of Philip J. Pauly’s Controlling Life: Jacques Loeb and the Engineering Ideal in Biology. The Behavior Analyst, 11, 205–207.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. MacCorquodale, K. (1970). On Chomsky’s review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 83–99.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Malagodi, E. F. (1986). On radicalizing behaviorism: A call for cultural analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 9, 1–17.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Marr, M. J. (1985). ’Tis the gift to be simple: A retrospective appreciation of Mach’s The Science of Mechanics. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 44, 129–138.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Midgley, B. D., & Morris, E. K. (1992). Nature = f(nurture): A review of Oyama’s The Ontogeny of Information: Developmental Systems and Evolution. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 229–240.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Morris, E. K. (1991). Deconstructing technological to a fault. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 411–416.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Morris, E. K. (1992). The aim, progress, and evolution of behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 15, 3–29.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Moxley, R. A. (1992). From mechanistic to functional behaviorism. American Psychologist, 47, 1300–1311.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Palmer, D. E. (1929). Jacques Loeb: A contribution to the history of psychology. Journal of General Psychology, 2, 97–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Palmer, D. C., & Donahoe, J. W. (1992). Essentialism and selectionism in cognitive science and behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47, 1344–1358.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Pauly, P. J. (1987). Controlling life: Jacques Loeb and the engineering ideal in biology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Provine, R. R. (1988). A hierarchy of developmental contingencies: A review of Purves and Lichtman’s Principles of Neural Development. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 565–569.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Purves, D., & Lichtman, J. W. (1985). Principles of neural development. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
  33. Schwartz, B., Schuldenfrei, R., & Lacey, H. (1978). Operant psychology as factory psychology. Behaviorism, 6, 229–254.Google Scholar
  34. Skinner, B. F. (1931). The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 427–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  36. Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review, 52, 270–277, 291–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Skinner, B. F. (1947). Experimental psychology. In W. Dennis (Ed.), Current trends in psychology (pp. 16–49). Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  39. Skinner, B. F. (1956). A case history in the scientific method. American Psychologist, 11, 221–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  42. Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  43. Skinner, B. F. (1979). The shaping of a behaviorist. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  44. Skinner, B. F. (1983). A matter of consequences. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  45. Smith, L. D. (1992). On prediction and control: B. F. Skinner and the technological ideal of science. American Psychologist, 47, 216–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Thompson, T. (1984). The examining magistrate for nature: A retrospective review of Claude Bernard’s An Introduction to Experimental Medicine. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 211–216.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of FloridaGainesvilleUSA

Personalised recommendations