The Behavior Analyst

, Volume 18, Issue 1, pp 173–185 | Cite as

“Reinforcement” in Behavior Theory

  • William N. Schoenfeld
Encore

Abstract

In its Pavlovian context, “reinforcement” was actually a descriptive term for the functional relation between an unconditional and a conditional stimulus. When it was adopted into operant conditioning, “reinforcement” became the central concept and the key operation, but with new qualifications, new referents, and new expectations. Some behavior theorists believed that “reinforcers” comprise a special and limited class of stimuli or events, and they speculated about what the essential “nature of reinforcement” might be. It is now known that any stimulus can serve a reinforcing function, with due recognition of such parameters as subject species characteristics, stimulus intensity, sensory modality, and schedule of application. This paper comments on these developments from the standpoint of reflex behavior theory.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bersh, P. J. (1951). The influence of two variables upon the establishment of a secondary reinforcer for operant responses. J. Exp. Psychol. 41, 62–73.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bugelski, R. R. (1938). Extinction with and without sub-goal reinforcement. J. Comp. Psychol. 26, 121–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Estes, W. K. (1944). An experimental study of punishment. Psychol. Monogr. 57, No. 263.Google Scholar
  4. Ferster, C. B. and Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Gantt, W. H. Personal communication.Google Scholar
  6. Glaser, R. (Ed.). (1971). The nature of reinforcement. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  7. Guthrie, E. R. (1930). Conditioning as a principle of learning. Psychol. Rev. 37, 412–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Guthrie, E. R. (1952). The psychology of learning. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
  9. Hall, J. F. (1976). Classical conditioning and instrumental learning. Philadelphia: Lippincott.Google Scholar
  10. Hilgard, E. R. (1956). Theories of Learning (2nd ed). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  11. Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  12. Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of Psychology. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  13. Konorski, J., & Miller, S. (1937a). On two types of conditioned reflex. J. Gen. Psychol. 16, 264–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Konorski, J., & Miller, S. (1937b). Further remarks on two types of conditioned reflex. J. Gen. Psychol. 17, 405–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Libby, A. (1951). Two variables in the acquisition of depressant properties by a stimulus. J. Exp. Psychol. 42, 100–107.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Miller, N. E. (1951). Learnable drives and rewards. In Stevens, S. S. (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
  17. Mueller, C. G., Jr., & Schoenfeld, W. N. Edwin R. Guthrie. (1954). In Estes W. K. et al. Modern learning theory. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  18. Reese, E. P. (1978). Human behavior: Analysis and application (2nd. ed.). Dubuque: Wm. C. Brown.Google Scholar
  19. Schoenfeld, W. N. (1966). Some old work for modern behavior theory. Cond. Reflex, 1, 219–223.Google Scholar
  20. Schoenfeld, W. N. (1972). Problems of modern behavior theory. Cond. Reflex 7, 33–65.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Schoenfeld, W. N. (1976). The “response” in behavior theory. Pavlov. J. Biol. Sci. 11, 129–149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Schoenfeld, W. N., Antonius, J. J., & Bersh, P. J. (1950). A preliminary study of training conditions necessary for secondary reinforcement. J. Exp. Psychol. 40, 40–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Schoenfeld, W. N., & Cole, B. K. (1972). Stimulus schedules: The t-t systems. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  24. Schoenfeld, W. N., & Cole, B. K. (1975). What is a “schedule of reinforcement”? Pavlov. J. Biol. Sci. 10, 52–61.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Schoenfeld, W. N., Cole, B. K., Lang, J., & Mankoff, R. (1973). “Contingency” in behavior theory. In F. J. McGuigan & D. B. Lumsden (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to conditioning and learning, V. H. Winston: Washington, D. C.Google Scholar
  26. Skinner, B. F. (1931). The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. J. Gen. Psychol. 5, 427–458. See also the added foreword to this paper when reprinted in B. F. Skinner, Cumulative Record, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1959, pp. 319–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Skinner, B. F. (1937). Two types of conditioned reflex: A reply to Konorski and Miller. J. Gen. Psychol. 16, 272–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  29. Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  30. Warden, C. J. et al. (1931). Animal motivation. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Wike, E. L. (1966). Secondary reinforcement. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  32. Zeaman, D. (1949). Response latency as a function of amount of reinforcement. J. Exp. Psychol. 39, 466–483.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Zeaman, D., & House, B. J. (1950). Response latency at zero drive after varying numbers of reinforcements. J Exp. Psychol. 40, 570–583.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • William N. Schoenfeld
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Queens CollegeCity University of New YorkNew YorkUSA
  2. 2.Cornell University Medical CollegeNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations