Advertisement

The Behavior Analyst

, Volume 16, Issue 1, pp 9–23 | Cite as

Separating Discriminative and Function-Altering Effects of Verbal Stimuli

  • Henry D. SchlingerJr.
Article

Abstract

Ever since Skinner’s first discussion of rule-governed behavior, behavior analysts have continued to define rules, either explicitly or implicitly, as verbal discriminative stimuli. Consequently, it is not difficult to find, in the literature on rule-governed behavior, references to stimulus control, antecedent control, or to rules occasioning behavior. However, some verbal stimuli have effects on behavior that are not easily described as discriminative. Such stimuli don’t evoke behavior as discriminative stimuli, but rather alter the functions of other stimuli in a manner analogous to operant and respondent conditioning. Hence, this type of control has been called function altering. Any known stimulus function (e.g., evocative, or [conditioned] reinforcing or punishing functions) can apparently be altered by such function-altering stimuli. Describing these stimuli as discriminative stimuli obscures their possible function-altering effects and consequently may retard inquiry into them. This paper encourages behavior analysts to begin separating the discriminative and function-altering effects of verbal stimuli and suggests that by doing so, behavior analysts may better understand what may be most unique about these stimuli. Results from several experiments, especially those in which children served as subjects, are analyzed. Finally, some speculations are offered concerning the genesis of function-altering stimuli.

Key words

rules rule-governed behavior function-altering stimuli contingency-specifying stimuli discriminative stimuli 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alessi, G. (1992). Models of proximate and ultimate causation in psychology. The American Psychologist, 47, 1359–1370.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. H. (1964). Reinforcement and instructions with mental patients. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 327–331.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Baer, R. A., Detrich, R., & Weninger, J. M. (1988). On the functional role of the verbalization in correspondence training procedures. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 345–356.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Baron, A., & Galizio, M. (1983). Instructional control of human operant behavior. The Psychological Record, 33, 495–520.Google Scholar
  5. Baxter, G. A., & Schlinger, H. (1990). Performance of children under a multiple random-ratio random-interval schedule of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 263–271.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Blakely, E., & Schlinger, H. (1987). Rules: Function-altering contingency-specifying stimuli. The Behavior Analyst, 10, 183–187.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Boren, J. J., & Devine, D. D. (1968). The repeated acquisition of behavioral chains. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 651–660.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Braam, C., & Malott, R. W. (1990). “I’ll do it when the snow melts”: The effects of deadlines and delayed outcomes on rule-governed behavior in preschool children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 67–76.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Brewer, W. F. (1974). There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In W. B. Weimer & D. S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes (pp. 1–42). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Brownstein, A. J., & Shull, R. L. (1985). A rule for the use of the term, “Rule-governed behavior.” The Behavior Analyst, 8, 265–267.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Buskist, W. F., Bennett, R. H., & Miller, H. L. (1981). Effects of instructional constraints on human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 217–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Catania, A. C., Matthews, B. A., & Shimoff, E. (1990). Properties of rule-governed behavior and their implications. In D. E. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behavior analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 215–230). Hove, England: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Cerutti, D. T. (1989). Discrimination theory of rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 259–276.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Verbal Behavior by B. F. Skinner. Language, 35, 26–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Danforth, J. S., Chase, P. N., Dolan, M., & Joyce, J. H. (1990). The establishment of stimulus control by instructions and by differential reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 87–112.Google Scholar
  16. Deacon, J. R., & Konarski, E. A. (1987). Correspondence training: An example of rule-governed behavior? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 391–400.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Devaney, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hayes, S. C. (1986). The case of the silent dog–Verbal reports and the analysis of rules. A review of Ericson and Simon’s Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 351–363.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Hayes, S. C., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Hayes, L. J. (1991). The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 119–137.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Malott, R. W. (1989). The achievement of evasive goals: Control by rules describing contingencies that are not direct acting. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 269–322). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Matthews, B. A., Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., & Sagvolden, T. (1977). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity to ratio and interval contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 453–467.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Michael, J. (1980). The discriminative stimulus or SD. The Behavior Analyst, 3, 47–49.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Michael, J. (1983). Evocative and repertoire-altering effects of an environmental event. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2, 19–21.Google Scholar
  25. Michael, J. (1985). Two kinds of verbal behavior plus a possible third. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 3, 1–4.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Michael, J. (1986). Repertoire-altering effects of remote contingencies. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 4, 10–18.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Mistr, K. (1992). The evocative and repertoire-altering effects of contingency-specifying stimuli. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of North Texas, Denton.Google Scholar
  28. Palmer, D. C. (1991). A behavioral interpretation of memory. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 261–279). Reno, NV: Context Press.Google Scholar
  29. Palmer, D. C., & Donahoe, J. W. (1992). Essentialism and selectionism in cognitive science and behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47, 1344–1358.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Pouthas, V., Droit, S., Jacquet, A.-Y., & Wearden, J. H. (1990). Temporal differentiation of response duration in children of different ages: Developmental changes in verbal and nonverbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 21–31.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Schlinger, H. D. (1990). A reply to behavior analysts writing about rules and rule-governed behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 77–82.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Schlinger, H. D. (1992). Theory in behavior analysis: An application to child development. American Psychologist. 47, 1396–1410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Schlinger, H., & Blakely, E. (1987). Function-altering effects of contingency-specifying stimuli. The Behavior Analyst, 10, 41–45.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Schlinger, H. D., Blakely, E., Fillhard, J., & Poling, A. (1991). Defining terms in behavior analysis: Reinforcer and discriminative stimulus. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 153–161.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5–13.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Skinner, B. F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem-solving. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving: Research, method, and theory (pp. 225–257). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  38. Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  39. Vaughan, M. E. (1985). Repeated acquisition in the analysis of rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 175–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wulfert, E., Dougher, M. J., & Greenway, D. E. (1991). Protocol analysis of the correspondence of verbal behavior and equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 489–504.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Zeiler, M. D. (1986). Behavioral units: A historical introduction. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 1–12). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Henry D. SchlingerJr.
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyWestern New England CollegeSpringfieldUSA

Personalised recommendations