Advertisement

The Behavior Analyst

, Volume 14, Issue 2, pp 95–105 | Cite as

Analyzing the Reinforcement Process at the Human Level: Can Application and Behavioristic Interpretation Replace Laboratory Research?

  • Alan Baron
  • Michael Perone
  • Mark Galizio
Article

Abstract

Critics have questioned the value of human operant conditioning experiments in the study of fundamental processes of reinforcement. Contradictory results from human and animal experiments have been attributed to the complex social and verbal history of the human subject. On these grounds, it has been contended that procedures that mimic those conventionally used with animal subjects represent a “poor analytic preparation” for the explication of reinforcement principles. In defending the use of conventional operant methods for human research, we make three points: (a) Historical variables play a critical role in research on processes of reinforcement, regardless of whether the subjects are humans or animals. (b) Techniques are available for detecting, analyzing, and counteracting such historical and extra-experimental influences; these include long-term observations, steady state designs, and, when variables are not amenable to direct control (e.g., age, gender, species), selection of subjects with common characteristics. (c) Other forms of evidence that might be used to validate conditioning principles—applied behavior analysis and behavioristic interpretation—have inherent limitations and cannot substitute for experimental analysis. We conclude that human operant conditioning experiments are essential for the analysis of the reinforcement process at the human level, but caution that their value depends on the extent to which the traditional methods of the experimental analysis of behavior are properly applied.

Keywords

human operant research reinforcement principles behavioral history individual differences steady-state methods applied behavior analysis behavioristic interpretation 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Azrin, N. H. (1958). Some effects of noise on human behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 183–200.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Bachrach, A. J. (1981). Psychological research (4th ed.). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  3. Baer, D. M. (1981). A flight of behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 4, 85–91.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some current dimensions of behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  6. Baron, A., & Galizio, M. (1976). Clock control of human performance on avoidance and fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 165–180.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Baron, A., & Galizio, M. (1983). Instructional control of human operant behavior. The Psychological Record, 33, 495–520.Google Scholar
  8. Baron, A., & Journey, J. W. (1989). Reinforcement of human reaction time: Manual-vocal differences. The Psychological Record, 39, 285–296.Google Scholar
  9. Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. (1966). Human free-operant avoidance of “time-out” from monetary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 557–565.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Baron, A., & Perone, M. (1982). The place of the human subject in the operant laboratory. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 143–158.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Barrett, J. E. (1977). Behavioral history as a determinant of the effects of d-amphetamine on punished behavior. Science, 198, 67–69.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Barrett, J. E. (1986). Behavioral history: Residual influences on subsequent behavior and drug effects. In N. Krasnegor & D. B. Gray (Eds.), Developmental behavioral pharmacology: Vol. V. Advances in behavioral pharmacology (pp. 99–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Bernstein, D. J. (1988). Laboratory lore and research practices in the experimental analysis of human behavior: Designing session logistics—How long, how often, how many? The Behavior Analyst, 11, 51–58.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Brewer, W. F. (1974). There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In W. B. Weimer & D. S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes (pp. 1–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Buskist, W., & Miller, H. L. (1982). The study of human operant behavior, 1958–1981: A topical bibliography. The Psychological Record, 32, 249–268.Google Scholar
  16. Catania, A. C., Matthews, B. A., & Shimoff, E. (1982). Instructed versus shaped human verbal behavior: Interactions with nonverbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 233–248.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Chomsky, N. (1959). [Review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal behavior]. Language, 35, 26–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Christie, R. (1951). Experimental naivete and experiential naivete. Psychological Bulletin, 48, 327–339.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Crossman, E. (1983). Las Vegas knows better. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 109–110.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Davey, G. (1988). Trends in human operant theory. In G. Davey & C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 1–14). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  21. Davey, G., & Cullen, C. (Eds.). (1988). Human operant conditioning and behavior modification. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  22. Deitz, S. M. (1978). Current status of applied behavior analysis: Science versus technology. American Psychologist, 33, 805–814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Deitz, S. M. (1982). Defining applied behavior analysis: An historical analogy. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 53–64.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Deitz, S. M. (1983). Two correct definitions of “applied.” The Behavior Analyst, 6, 105–106.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Denny, M. R., & Ratner, S. C. (1970). Comparative psychology: Research in animal behavior (rev. ed.). Homewood, NJ: Dorsey.Google Scholar
  26. Dinsmoor, J. A. (1983). Observing and conditioned reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 693–728. (Includes commentary)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Egli, M., & Thompson, T. (1989). Effects of methadone on alternative fixed-ratio fixed-interval performance: Latent influences on schedule-controlled responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 141–153.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Epling, W. F., & Pierce, W. D. (1983). Applied behavior analysis: New directions from the laboratory. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 27–37.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Estes, W. K. (1972). Reinforcement in human behavior. American Scientist, 60, 723–729.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Ferster, C. B. (1953). The use of the free operant in the analysis of behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 50, 263–274.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Galizio, M., & Buskist, W. (1988). Laboratory lore and research practices in the experimental analysis of human behavior: Selecting reinforcers and arranging contingencies. The Behavior Analyst, 11, 65–69.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Hake, D. F. (1982). The basic-applied continuum and the possible evolution of human operant social and verbal research. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 21–28.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Hayes, S. C. (1987a). Upward and downward continuity: It’s time to change our strategic assumptions. Behavior Analysis, 22, 3–6.Google Scholar
  34. Hayes, S. C. (1987b). Language and the incompatibility of evolutionary and psychological continuity. Behavior Analysis, 22, 49–54.Google Scholar
  35. Hayes, S. C., Rincover, A., & Solnick, J. V. (1980). The technical drift of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 275–285.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  37. Hineline, P. N. (1986). The relationship between subject and experimenter. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 123–126.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Holland, J. G. (1958). Human vigilance. Science, 128, 61–63.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Jenkins, H. M., & Harrison, R. H. (1960). Effect of discrimination training on auditory generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 246–253.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Laties, V. G., & Weiss, B. (1960). Human observing behavior after signal detection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3, 27–33.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Lowe, C. F. (1979). Determinants of human operant behavior. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Advances in analysis of behaviour: Vol. 1. Reinforcement and the organization of behaviour (pp. 159–192). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  42. Lowe, C. F. (1983). Radical behaviorism and human psychology. In G. C. L. Davey (Ed.), Animal models of human behavior (pp. 71–93). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  43. McPherson, A., Bonem, M., Green, G., & Osborne, J. G. (1984). A citation analysis of the influence on research of Skinner’s Verbal behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 7, 157–167.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Menich, S. R., & Baron, A. (1984). Social housing of rats: Life-span effects on reaction time, exploration, weight, and longevity. Experimental Aging Research, 10, 95–100.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Michael, J. (1980). Flight from behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 3, 1–21.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Michael, J. (1987). The experimental analysis of human behavior: History, current status and future directions. Comments by the discussant. The Psychological Record, 37, 37–42.Google Scholar
  47. Miller, H. L., Jr. (1983). More than promissory? Reflections on the once and future experimental analysis of human behavior. The Psychological Record, 33, 551–564.Google Scholar
  48. Morris, E. K., Johnson, L. M., Todd, J. T., & Higgins, S. T. (1988). Laboratory lore and research practices in the experimental analysis of human behavior: Subject selection. The Behavior Analyst, 11, 43–50.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. Perone, M. (1985). On the impact of human operant research: Asymmetrical patterns of cross-citation between human and nonhuman research. The Behavior Analyst, 8, 185–189.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  50. Perone, M. (1988). Laboratory lore and research practices in the experimental analysis of human behavior: Use and abuse of subjects’ verbal reports. The Behavior Analyst, 11, 71–75.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  51. Perone, M. (in press). Experimental design in the analysis of free-operant behavior. In I. H. Iversen & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), Techniques in the behavioral and neural sciences: Experimental analysis of behavior. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  52. Perone, M., & Baron, A. (1980). Reinforcement of human observing behavior by a stimulus correlated with extinction or increased effort. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 239–261.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Perone, M., Galizio, M., & Baron, A. (1988). The relevance of animal-based principles in the laboratory study of human operant conditioning. In G. Davey & C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 59–85). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  54. Pierce, W. D., & Epling, W. F. (1980). What happened to analysis in applied behavior analysis? The Behavior Analyst, 3, 1–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  55. Pilgrim, C., & Johnston, J. M. (1988). Laboratory lore and research practices in the experimental analysis of human behavior: Issues in instructing subjects. The Behavior Analyst, 11, 59–64.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  56. Poling, A. (1984). Comparing humans to other species: We’re animals and they’re not infrahumans. The Behavior Analyst, 7, 211–212.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Poling, A., Picker, M., Grossen, D., Hall-Johnson, E., & Holbrook, M. (1981). The schism between experimental and applied behavior analysis: Is it real and who cares? The Behavior Analyst, 4, 93–102.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  58. Poppen, R. (1982). The fixed-interval scallop in human affairs. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 127–136.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. Rachlin, H. (1989). Judgment, decision, and choice: A cognitive/behavioral synthesis. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.Google Scholar
  60. Reynolds, G. S. (1961). Attention in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 203–208.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  61. Schwartz, B., & Lacey, H. (1988). What applied studies of human operant conditioning tell us about humans and about operant conditioning. In G. Davey & C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 27–42). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  62. Shull, R. L., Lawrence, P. S., Tota, M. E., Sharp, J. A., Drusdow, M. A., Torquato, R. D., & Soyars, V. A. (1989). [Review of Davey and Cullen’s Human operant conditioning and behavior modification]. The Behavior Analyst, 12, 69–77.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  63. Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  64. Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  65. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  66. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Skinner, B. F. (1968). The technology of teaching. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  68. Skinner, B. F. (1975). Comment on Watt’s “B. F. Skinner and the technological control of social behavior.” The American Political Science Review, 69, 228–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Skinner, B. F. (1978). Reflections on behaviorism and society. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  70. Skinner, B. F. (1983). A matter of consequences. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  71. Spielberger, C. D., & DeNike, L. D. (1966). Descriptive behaviorism versus cognitive theory in verbal conditioning experiments. Psychological Review, 73, 306–326.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Thomas, D. (1969). The use of operant conditioning techniques to investigate perceptual processes in animals. In R. M. Gilbert & N. S. Sutherland (Eds.), Animal discrimination learning (pp. 1–33). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  73. Urbain, C., Poling, A., Millam, J., & Thompson, T. (1978). d-amphetamine and fixed-interval performance: Effects of operant history. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 385–392.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  74. Wanchisen, B. A. (1990). Forgetting the lessons of history. The Behavior Analyst, 13, 31–37.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  75. Wanchisen, B. A., Tatham, T. A., & Mooney, S. E. (1989). Variable-ratio conditioning history produces high- and low-rate fixed-interval performance in rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 167–179.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  76. Wearden, J. H. (1988). Some neglected problems in the analysis of human operant behavior. In G. Davey & C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant conditioning and behavior modification (pp. 197–224). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  77. Weiner, H. (1962). Some effects of response cost upon human operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 201–208.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  78. Weiner, H. (1964). Conditioning history and human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 383–385.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  79. Weiner, H. (1969). Controlling human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 349–373.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  80. Weiner, H. (1983). Some thoughts on discrepant human-animal performances under schedules of reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 33, 521–532.Google Scholar
  81. Woods, T. S. (1980). On the alleged incompatibility of analysis and application: A response to Pierce and Epling. The Behavior Analyst, 3, 67–69.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Alan Baron
    • 1
  • Michael Perone
    • 2
  • Mark Galizio
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Wisconsin-MilwaukeeMilwaukeeUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyWest Virginia UniversityMorgantownUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of North Carolina at WilmingtonWilmingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations