The Behavior Analyst

, Volume 14, Issue 1, pp 49–60 | Cite as

Skinner and Chomsky 30 Years Later Or: The Return of the Repressed

  • Julie Andresen
Article

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Andresen, Julie. (1988). Review of Paul Friedrich The Language Parallax. Linguistic Relativism and Poetic Indeterminacy (1986). Language in Society, 17(4), 600–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andresen, Julie. (1990). Linguistics in America 1769–1924: A critical history. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  3. Austin, John Langham. (1911–1960). (1975 [1962]). How to do things with words, 2nd ed. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bullock, T. H. (1967). “Signals and neuronal coding.” The neurosciences: A study program ed. by Gardner Q. Quarton, Theodore Melnechuck, Francis O. Schmitt and the associate and staff of the Neurosciences Research Program, pp. 347–352. New York: Rockefeller University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Chase, Philip, & Parrott, Linda, Eds. (1986). Psychological aspects of language. The West Virginia Lectures. Springfield, Ill.: Charles Thomas.Google Scholar
  6. Chomsky, Noam. (1959). Review of Skinner (1957). Language, 35 26–58. (Repr. with introduction in Readings in the psychology of language ed. by Leon A. Jakobovits & Murray Miron (pp. 142–171). Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1967.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chomsky, Noam. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
  8. Chomsky, Noam. (1971). The case against B. F. Skinner. Review of Beyond Freedom and Dignity (New York: A. A. Knopf, 1971), by B. F. Skinner. The New York Review of Books, 17, 18–24.Google Scholar
  9. Chomsky, Noam. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Chomsky, Noam. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin and use. New York-Westport-London: Praeger.Google Scholar
  11. Chomsky, Noam. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge. The Managua lectures. Cambridge, Mass. & London: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
  12. Dreyfus, Hubert, & Dreyfus, Stuart. (1988). Making a mind versus modeling the brain: Artificial intelligence back at a branchpoint. Daedalus, 117(1), 15–43.Google Scholar
  13. Edelman, Gerald. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  14. Gleick, James. (1987). Chaos: Making a new science. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin.Google Scholar
  15. Hopper, Paul. (1988). Emergent grammar and the a priori grammar postulate. In Deborah Tannen (Ed.), Linguistics in context: Connecting observation and understanding, lectures from the 1985 LSA/TESOL and NEH Institutes (pp. 117–134). Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.Google Scholar
  16. Koerner, E. F. K. (1983). “The Chomskyan Revolution” and its historiography. Language and Communication, 3, 147–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. MacWhinney, Brian, & Leinbach, Jared. (1989). Language learning: Cues or rules? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 255–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Marr, David. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  19. Newmeyer, Frederick J., & Emonds, Joseph. (1971). The linguist in American society. Papers from the Seventh Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 285–303). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
  20. Papert, Seymour. (1988). One AI or many? Daedalus, 117(1), 1–14.Google Scholar
  21. Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–193.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Reeke, George, & Edelman, Gerald. (1988). Real brains and artificial intelligence. Daedalus, 117(1), 143–173.Google Scholar
  23. Rosenfield, Israel. (1988). The invention of memory. A new view of the brain. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  24. Rumelhart, David, & McClelland, James, & the PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructures of cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
  25. Skinner, B. F. (1938). Behavior of organisms. New York-London: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
  26. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Skinner, B. F. (1966). The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Science, 153, 1205–1213.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Skinner, B. F. (1972). A lecture on ‘having’ a poem. Cumulative record: A selection of papers, 3rd ed., pp. 345–355. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  29. Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, 501–504.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Smith, Barbara Herrnstein. (1957). A new anatomy of language, Unpublished manuscript review of Skinner (1957).Google Scholar
  31. Tikhomirov, O. K. (1959). Review of Skinner (1957). Word, 15, 362–367.Google Scholar
  32. Vargas, E. A. (1986). Intraverbal behavior. In Chase & Parrott (pp. 128–151).Google Scholar
  33. Vološinov, Valentin N. (1973 [1929]). Marxism and the philosophy of language, translated by Ladislav Matejka and I. R. Titunik. Cambridge, London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Julie Andresen
    • 1
  1. 1.Duke UniversityDurhamUSA

Personalised recommendations