The Behavior Analyst

, Volume 11, Issue 2, pp 115–129 | Cite as

Toward an Explicit Analysis of Generalization: A Stimulus Control Interpretation

  • Kimberly C. Kirby
  • Warren K. Bickel
Article

Abstract

Producing generality of treatment effects to new settings has been a critical concern for applied behavior analysts, but a systematic and reliable means of producing generality has yet to be provided. We argue that the principles of stimulus control and reinforcement underlie the production of most generalized effects; therefore, we suggest interpreting generalization programming in terms of stimulus control. The generalization programming procedures identified by Stokes and Baer (1977) are discussed in terms of both the stimulus control tactics explicitly identified and those that may be operating but are not explicitly identified. Our interpretation clarifies the critical components of Stokes and Baer’s procedures and places greater emphasis on planning for generalization as a part of training procedures.

Key words

generalized effects generalization programming maintenance quantal interpretation stimulus control stimulus generalization 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ayllon, T., & Azrin, N. (1968). The token economy: A motivational system for therapy and rehabilitation. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, S. R., & Spradlin, J. E. (1980). The generalized effects of productive labeling training involving common object classes. Journal of the Association for the Severely Handicapped, 5(2), 143–157.Google Scholar
  3. Baer, D. M. (1982a). The role of current pragmatics in the future analysis of generalization technology. In R. B. Stuart (Ed.), Adherence, compliance, and generalization in behavioral medicine (pp. 192–212). New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
  4. Baer, D. M. (1982b). Applied behavior analysis. In G. T. Wilson & C. M. Franks (Eds.), Contemporary behavior therapy (pp. 277–309). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  5. Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Baer, R. A., Williams, J. A., Osnes, P. G., & Stokes, T. F. (1984). Delayed reinforcement as an indiscriminable contingency in verbal/nonverbal correspondence training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 429–440.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Becker, W. C., & Carnine, D. W. (1981). Direct instruction. In S. W. Bijou & R. Ruiz (Eds.), Behavior modification: Contributions to education (pp. 184–210). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Becker, W. C., & Engelmann, S. (1978). Systems for basic instruction: Theory and applications. In A. C. Catania & T. A. Brigham (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis: Social and instructional processes (pp. 325–377). New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
  9. Bickel, W. K. (1987). The quantal interpretation of stimulus control. The Psychological Record, 37, 155–159.Google Scholar
  10. Bickel, W. K., & Etzel, B. C. (1985). The quantal nature of controlling stimulus-response relations as measured in tests of stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 44, 245–270.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Bickel, W. K., Richmond, G., Bell, J., & Brown, K. (1986). A micro-analysis of the controlling stimulus-response relations engendered during the assessment of stimulus overselectivity. The Psychological Record, 36, 225–238.Google Scholar
  12. Bickel, W. K., Stella, M. E., & Etzel, B. C. (1984). A re-evaluation of stimulus overselectivity: Restricted stimulus control or stimulus control hierarchies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 14, 137–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Blough, D. S. (1963). Interresponse time as a function of continuous variables: A new method and some data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 237–246.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Campbell, C. R., & Stremal-Campbell, K. (1982). Programming “loose training” as a strategy to facilitate language generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 295–301.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Catania, A. C., & Cutts, D. (1963). Experimental control of superstitious responding in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 203–208.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1965). The complex discriminated operant: Studies of matching-to-sample and related problems. In D. M. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization (pp. 284–330). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Deitz, S. M. (1978). Current status of applied behavior analysis: Science versus technology. American Psychologist, 33, 805–814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dixon, L. S., Spradlin, J. E., Girardeau, F. L., & Etzel, B. C. (1974). Facilitating the acquisition of an “in front” spatial discrimination. Acta Symbolica, 5(4), 1–21.Google Scholar
  19. Dixon, M. H. (1978). Teaching receptive classes with receptive label training. Acta Symbolica, 9, 17–35.Google Scholar
  20. Dixon, M. H., & Spradlin, J. E. (1976). Establishing stimulus equivalences among retarded adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 144–164.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Engelmann, S., & Carnine, D. W. (1982). Theory of instruction: Principles and applications. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
  22. Etzel, B. C. (1987). Pigeons and children: What are the differences? The Psychological Record, 37, 17–27.Google Scholar
  23. Ferster, C. B., & Hammer, C. E. (1966). Synthesizing the components of arithmetic behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 634–676). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  24. Fowler, S. A., & Baer, D. M. (1981). Do I have to be good all day? The timing of delayed reinforcement as a factor in generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 13–24.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Garcia, E. (1974). The training and generalization of a conversational speech form in nonverbal retardates. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 137–149.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Glynn, E. L., & Thomas, J. D. (1974). Effect of cueing on self-control of classroom behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 299–306.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Goetz, E. M., & Baer, D.M. (1973). Social control of form diversity and the emergence of new forms in children’s blockbuilding. Journal of Applied Behavior A nalysis, 6, 105–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Griffiths, H., & Craighead, W. E. (1972). Generalization in operant speech therapy for misarticulation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 37, 485–494.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Guttman, N., & Kalish, H. I. (1956). Discriminability and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 79–88.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Halle, J. W., Baer, D. M., & Spradlin, J. E. (1981). Teachers’ generalized use of delay as a stimulus control procedure to increase language use in handicapped children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 389–409.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Herbert, E. W., & Baer, D. M. (1972). Training parents as behavior modifiers: Self-recording of contingent attention. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 139–149.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Hively, W. (1962). Programming stimuli in matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 279–298.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Holman, J., & Baer, D. M. (1979). Facilitating generalization of on-task behavior through self-monitoring of academic tasks. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9(4), 429–445.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Iversen, I. H., Sidman, M., & Carrigan, P. (1986). Stimulus definition in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 297–304.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Johnston, J. M. (1979). On the relation between generalization and generality. The Behavior Analyst, 2, 1–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Koegel, R. L., & Rincover, A. (1974). Treatment of psychotic children in a classroom environment: I. Learning in a large group. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 45–59.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. MacDonald, R. P. F. (1983). Match-to-sample performance in the context of group teaching (Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas ).Google Scholar
  38. Marholin, D., II, & Seigel, L. J. (1976). Beyond the law of effect: Programming for the maintenance of behavior change. In D. Marholin II (Ed.), Child behavior therapy (pp. 397–415). New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
  39. Marholin, D., II, Seigel, L. J., & Phillips, D. (1976). Treatment and transfer: A search for empirical procedures. In M. Hersen, R. M. Eisler, & P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in behavior modification (Vol. 3, pp. 293–341). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  40. Marholin, D., II, & Touchette, P. E. (1979). The role of stimulus control and response consequences. In A. P. Goldstein & F. H. Kanfer (Eds.), Maximizing treatment gains: Transfer enhancement in psychology (pp. 303–351). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  41. Morse, W. H., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). A second type of “superstition” in the pigeon. American Journal of Psychology, 70, 308–311.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Nevin, J. A. (1973). Stimulus control. In J. A. Nevin & R. S. Reynolds (Eds.), The study of behavior (pp. 115–152). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
  43. Rand, J. F. (1977). Behaviors observed during S- in a simple discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 103–117.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Ray, B. A., & Sidman, M. (1970). Reinforcement schedules and stimulus control. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp. 187–214). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  45. Rilling, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory process. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 432–480). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  46. Rincover, A., & Koegel, R.L. (1975). Setting generality and stimulus control in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8, 235–246.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Schwarz, M. L., & Hawkins, R. P. (1970). Application of delayed reinforcement procedures to the behavior of an elementary school child. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 3, 85–96.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Sidman, M. (1969). Generalization gradients and stimulus control in delayed matching-to-sample. Journal of the Experimental A nalysis of Behavior, 12, 745–757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing, 14, 5–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sidman, M. (1979). Remarks. Behaviorism, 1, 123–126.Google Scholar
  51. Sidman, M. (1980). A note on the measurement of conditional discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 285–289.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  52. Sidman, M., Cresson, O., & Willison-Morris, M. (1974). Acquisition of matching to sample via mediated transfer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 261–273.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  55. Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Skinner, B. F. (1965). Stimulus generalization in an operant: A historical note. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization (pp. 193–209). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Spradlin, J. E., & Dixon, M. H. (1976). Establishing conditional discriminations without direct training: Stimulus classes and labels. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 80(5), 555–561.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Stoddard, L. T., & McIlvane, B. (in press). Generalization after interdimensional discrimination training in two-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.Google Scholar
  59. Stoddard, L. T., & Sidman, M. (1971). The removal and restoration of stimulus control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 16, 143–154.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  60. Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1976). Preschool peers as mutual generalization-facilitating agents. Behavior Therapy, 7, 549–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349–367.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  62. Stokes, T. F., Baer, D. M., & Jackson, R. L. (1974). Programming the generalization of a greeting response in four retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 599–610.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  63. Stokes, T. F., Doud, C. L., Rowbury, T. G., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Peer facilitation of generalization in a preschool classroom. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6(2), 203–209.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Terrace, H. S. (1966). Stimulus control. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 271–344). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  65. Touchette, P. E. (1969). Tilted lines as complex stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 211–214.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  66. Walker, H. M., & Buckley, N. K. (1972). The use of positive reinforcement in conditioning attending behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 245–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kimberly C. Kirby
    • 1
  • Warren K. Bickel
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.School of MedicineJohns Hopkins UniversityBaltimoreUSA
  2. 2.School of MedicineUniversity of VermontBurlingtonUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of Vermont College of MedicineBurlingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations