Advertisement

The Behavior Analyst

, Volume 10, Issue 2, pp 183–187 | Cite as

Rules: Function-Altering Contingency-Specifying Stimuli

  • Elbert Blakely
  • Henry Schlinger
Article

Abstract

Behavior analysts have traditionally defined rules as discriminative stimuli. Three problems with this interpretation are discussed. First, because the effects of rules are often delayed, and the effects of discriminative stimuli are immediate, classifying rules as discriminative stimuli violates the definitional requirements of the latter. Second, when rules are defined as discriminative stimuli, other truly unique effects of rules may be obscured. Finally, both rules and contingencies develop new behavioral relations; however, when rules are interpreted as discriminative stimuli, their effects are not readily compared with those of contingencies. As an alternative, we suggest that rules be interpreted as function-altering contingency-specifying stimuli. Implications of this function-altering interpretation for terminology and research strategy are discussed.

Key words

rules rule-governed behavior contingency-specifying stimuli discriminative stimuli function-altering effects 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Baldwin, J. D., & Baldwin, J. I. (1981). Behavior principles in everyday life. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  2. Brewer, W. F. (1974). There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in normal, adult, human beings. In W. Weimer & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes (pp. 1–42). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Brownstein, A. J., & Shull, R. L. (1985). A rule for the use of the term, “Rule-governed behavior.” The Behavior Analyst, 8, 265–267.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J., & Austin, G. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  5. Catania, A.C. (1984). Learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  6. Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactical structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  7. Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Galizio, M. (1979). Contingency-shaped and rule-governed behavior: Instructional control of human loss avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 53–70.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Hayes, S. C. (1986). The case of the silent dog—verbal reports and the analysis of rules: A review of Ericsson and Simon’s protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 351–363.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A., Zettle, R. D., Rosenfarb, I., & Korn, Z. (1986). Rule-governed behavior and sensitivity to changing contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 237–256.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Mackintosh, N. J. (1977). Stimulus control: Attentional factors. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 481–513). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  12. Michael, J. (1980). The discriminative stimulus or SD. The Behavior Analyst, 3, 47–50.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Michael, J. (1983). Evocative and repertoire-altering effects of an environmental event. VB News, 2, 21–23.Google Scholar
  14. Michael, J. (1986). Repertoire-altering effects of remote contingencies. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 4, 10–18.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Palmer, D. C. (1986, June). A behavioral interpretation of memory. (Paper presented at the Summer Institute on Verbal Relations, Bad Kreuznach, Germany.)Google Scholar
  16. Reynolds, G. S. (1975). A primer of operant conditioning (rev. ed.). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
  17. Rilling, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 432–480). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  18. Schlinger, H., & Blakely, E. (1987). Function-altering effects of contingency-specifying stimuli. The Behavior Analyst, 10, 41–46.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., & Matthews, B. A. (1981). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity of low-rate performance to schedule contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 207–220.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Skinner, B. F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem-solving. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving: Research, method, teaching (pp. 225–257). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  21. Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  22. Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  23. Terrace, H. S. (1966). Stimulus control. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 271–344). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  24. Vaughan, M. E. (1985). Repeated acquisition in the analysis of rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Experimental A nalysis of Behavior, 44, 175–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Vaughan, M. E. (1987). Rule-governed behavior and higher mental processes. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), B.F. Skinner: Consensus and controversy (pp. 257–264). Barcombe, England: Falmer Press.Google Scholar
  26. Zuriff, G. E. (1985). Behaviorism: A conceptual reconstruction. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 1987

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elbert Blakely
    • 1
  • Henry Schlinger
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyWestern Michigan UniversityKalamazooUSA

Personalised recommendations