Skip to main content
Log in

The Place of the Human Subject in the Operant Laboratory

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although laboratory study of human behavior seems an obvious vehicle for strengthening the scientific base of behavior analysis, the place of the human subject within the operant laboratory remains problematic. The prevailing research strategy has been to link principles developed with animals to human affairs, either through interpretation of naturally occurring human behaviors or through application of the principles to the solution of human problems. The paucity of laboratory research on human operant behavior derives from several misconceptions: the possibility that experimental demand characteristics and pre-experimental behavioral dispositions of human subjects contaminate the results; that ethical considerations place undue constraint on research topics and experimental designs; and that uncontrollable variation in subjects’ histories and other relevant personal characteristics prevents observation of reliable functional relations. We argue that these problems do not pose insurmountable obstacles to the experimental analysis of human behavior; that adequate methods of control and analysis are available; and that operant techniques, by emphasizing experimentally imposed contingencies, are well suited for the laboratory study of human behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Reference Note

  1. Baron, A. The place of the human animal in the operant laboratory. In R. Poppen (Chair), Issues in human operant conditioning. Symposium presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, August 1975.

  2. Baron, A., & Galizio, M. Instructional control ofhuman operant responding. In P. Harzem and E. Ribes (Chairs), Language and behavior, International Symposium on the Science of Behavior, Mexico City, 1982.

  3. DeWaard, R. J., & Baron, A. Control of human behavior in the operant laboratory: Concurrent operant performances. In R. Poppen (Chair), The experimental analysis of human behavior. Symposium presented at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Dearborn, Michigan, June 1979.

  4. Perone, M., & Baron, A. Age related effects of response pacing on the repeated acquisition of behavioral chains. In J. M. Johnston (Chair), Experimental analyses of human behavior. Symposium presented at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee, Wisconsin, May 1981.

References

  • Ader, R., & Tatum, R. Free-operant avoidance conditioning in human subjects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 275–276.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H. Some effects of noise on human behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1958, 1, 183–200.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M. Escape and avoidance response of pre-school children to two schedules of reinforcement withdrawal. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1960, 3, 155–159.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. Some current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 91–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Principles of behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. The correlation-based law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1973, 20, 137–153.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W., Peterson, R. F., & Ault, M. H. A method to integrate descriptive and experimental field studies at the level of data and empirical concepts. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 175–191.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bolles, R. C. Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 32–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E., & Bevan, P. Behavior of humans in variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1976, 26, 135–141.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Branch, M. N., & Malagodi, E. F. Where have all the behaviorists gone? The Behavior Analyst, 1980, 3, 31–38.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W. F. There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In W. B. Weimer & D. S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and symbolic processes. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1974, 1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brogden, H. E. Some observations on two methods in psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 1972, 77, 431–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cahoon, D. D., & Crosby, C. C. A learning approach to chronic drug use: Sources of reinforcement. Behavior Therapy, 1972, 3, 64–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. Verbal Behavior. By B. F. Skinner. Language, 1959, 35, 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 1957, 12, 671–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeWaard, R. J. Matching and failure of response independence by human subjects on concurrent VI VI schedules of reinforcement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, 1980.

  • Ferster, C. B. The use of the free operant in analysis of behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 1953, 50, 263–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galizio, M. Contingency-shaped and rule-governed behavior: Instructional control of human loss avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1979, 31, 53–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E. R. The psychology of learning. New York: Harper, 1935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harzem, P., Lowe, C. F., & Bagshaw, M. Verbal control in human operant behavior. Psychological Record, 1978, 28, 405–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heron, W. T., & Skinner, B. F. The rate of extinction in maze-bright and maze-dull rats. Psychological Record, 1940, 4, 11–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hersen, M., & Barlow, D. H. Single case experimental designs. New York: Pergamon Press, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. G., & Skinner, B. F. The analysis of behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holz, W. C., & Azrin, N. H. Conditioning human verbal behavior. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoyer, W. J. Application of operant techniques to the modification of elderly behavior. Gerontologist, 1973, 13, 18–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulicka, I. M. Cognitive functioning in late adulthood. Master lecture series on the psychology of aging. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. The place of innate individual and species differences in a natural-science theory of behavior. Psychological Review, 1945, 52, 55–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. M., & Pennypacker, H. S. Strategies and tactics of human behavioral research. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelman, H. C. Human use of human subjects: The problem of deception in social psychological experiments. Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 67, 1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. Adult cognitive development: In search of alternative interpretations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1977, 23, 227–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leitenberg, H. Is time-out from positive reinforcement an aversive event? Psychological Bulletin, 1965, 64, 428–441.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, O. R. Operant conditioning methods in diagnosis. In J. H. Nodine & J. H. Moyer (Eds.), Psychosomatic medicine: The first Hahnemann symposium. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, O. R. Geriatric behavioral prosthetics. In R. Kastenbaum (Ed.), New thoughts on old age. New York: Springer, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F. Determinants of human operant behavior. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Reinforcement and the organization of behaviour. New York: Wiley, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. Positive and negative reinforcement, a distinction that is no longer necessary; or a better way to talk about bad things. Behaviorism, 1975, 3, 33–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. K. Some punishing effects of response-force. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1970, 13, 215–220.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Murrell, F. H. The effect of extensive practice on age differences in reaction time. Journal of Gerontology, 1970, 25, 268–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. L., & Myers, L. E. Undermatching: A reappraisal of performance on concurrent variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1911, 27, 203–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M. On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 1962, 17, 776–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perone, M., & Baron, A. Age-related effects of pacing on acquisition and performance of response sequences: An operant analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 1982, 37, 443–449.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. A review of M. J. Mahoney’s Cognition and Behavior Modification. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1977, 10, 369–374.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, R. E., & Strongman, K. T. Instruction-dependent facilitation during a pretimeout stimulus in human subjects. Psychonomic Science, 1970, 20, 348–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B., Schuldenfrei, R., & Lacey, H. Operant psychology as factory psychology. Behaviorism, 1978, 6, 229–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 406–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., & Matthews, B. A. Uninstructed human responding: Responsivity of low-rate performance to schedule contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1981, 36, 207–220.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shipley, C. R., Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. Effects of timeout from one reinforcer on human behavior maintained by another reinforcer. Psychological Record, 1972, 22, 201–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. Tactics of scientific research. New York: Basic Books, 1960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. Operant techniques. In A. J. Bachrach (Ed.), Experimental foundations of clinical psychology. New York: Basic Books, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. A case history in scientific method. American Psychologist, 1956, 11, 221–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Reply to Thouless. Australian Journal of Psychology, 1963, 15, 92–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. Comment on Watts’s “B. F. Skinner and the technological control of social behavior.” The American Political Science Review, 1975, 69, 228–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, G. C. Nondiscriminated avoidance behavior in human subjects. Science, 1961, 133, 641–642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thoresen, C. E., & Mahoney, M. J. Behavioral self-control. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thouless, R. H. The psychology teaching programme of Holland and Skinner. Australian Journal of Psychology, 1963, 15, 85–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanck, W. S. The operant, from rat to man. Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1955, 17, 594–601.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, M. W. B. F. Skinner and the technological control of social behavior. The American Political Science Review, 1975, 69, 214–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, H. Some effects of response cost upon human operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1962, 5, 201–208.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, J., & Baydan, N. T. Punishment of S-delta responding of humans in conditional matching to sample by time-out. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 6, 589–597.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Support was provided by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (Grant DA01080) and the National Institute on Aging (Grant AG02513).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Baron, A., Perone, M. The Place of the Human Subject in the Operant Laboratory. BEHAV ANALYST 5, 143–158 (1982). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392383

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392383

Navigation