Aging Clinical and Experimental Research

, Volume 19, Issue 5, pp 394–399 | Cite as

Does music enhance cognitive performance in healthy older adults? The Vivaldi effect

  • Nicola Mammarella
  • Beth Fairfield
  • Cesare Cornoldi
Original Articles

Abstract

Background and aims: Controversial evidence suggests that music can enhance cognitive performance. In the present study, we examined whether listening to an excerpt of Vivaldi’s “Four Seasons” had a positive effect on older adults’ cognitive performance in two working memory tasks. Methods: With a repeated-measures design, older adults were presented with the forward version of the digit span and phonemic fluency in classical music, white-noise and no-music conditions. Results: Classical music significantly increased working memory performance compared with the no-music condition. In addition, this effect did not occur with white noise. Conclusion: The authors discuss this finding in terms of the arousal-and-mood hypothesis and the role of working memory resources in aging.

Keywords

Aging music working memory 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Wallace WT. Memory for music: effect of melody on recall of text. J Exp Psychol: Learn, Mem & Cogn 1994; 20: 1471–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Schellenberg EG. Music and cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychol Sci 2005; 14: 317–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Fox LS, Knight BG, Zelinski E. Mood induction with older adults: A tool for investigating effects of depressed mood. Psychol Aging 1998; 13: 519–23.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Moreno R, Mayer RE. A Coherence effect in multimedia learning: the case for minimizing irrelevant sounds in the design of multimedia instructional messages. J Educ Psychol 2000; 92: 117–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Rauscher FH, Shaw G. Key components of the Mozart effect. Percept Mot Skills 1998; 86: 835–42.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Foster NA, Valentine ER. The effect of auditory stimulation on autobiographical recall in dementia. Exp Aging Res 2001; 27: 215–28.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Rauscher FH, Shaw GL, Ky KN. Music and spatial task performance. Nature 1993; 365: 611.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Nantais KM, Schellenberg EG. The Mozart effect: an artifact of preference. Psychol Sci 1999; 10: 370–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Steele KM, Brown JD. Failure to confirm the Rauscher and Shaw description of recovery of the Mozart effect. Percept Mot Skills 1999; 88: 843–8.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    McKelvie P, Low J. Listening to Mozart does not improve children’s spatial ability: final curtains for the Mozart effect. Brit J Dev Psychol 2002; 20: 241–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Steele KM, Bass KE, Crook, MD. The mystery of the Mozart effect: failure to replicate. Psychol Sci 1999; 10: 366–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Chabris CF, Steele KM, Dalla Bella S, et al. Prelude or requiem for the ‘Mozart effect’? Nature 1999; 400: 826–7.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Sherratt K, Thornton A, Hatton C. Music interventions for people with dementia: a review of the literature. Aging Ment Health 2004; 8: 3–12.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Irish M, Cunningham CJ, Walsh JB, et al. Investigating the enhancing effect of music on autobiographical memory in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006; 22:108–20.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Rahhal T, Hasher L, Colcombe SJ. Instructional manipulations and age differences in memory: now you see them, now you don’t. Psychol Aging 2001; 16: 697–707.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Eysenck MW. Arousal, learning and memory. Psychol Bull 1976; 83: 389–404.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Carlson S, Rama P, Artchakov D, Linnankoski I. Effects of music and white noise on working memory performance in monkeys. Neuroreport 1997; 8: 2853–6.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Rauscher FH, Robinson KD, Jens JJ. Improved maze learning through early music exposure in rats. Neurol Res 1998; 20: 427–32.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Thompson WF, Schellenberg EG, Husain G. Arousal, mood and the Mozart effect. Psych Science 2001; 12: 248–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Schulkind MD, Hennis LK, Rubin, D.C. Music, emotion, and autobiographical memory: they’re playing your song. Mem & Cogn 1999; 27: 948–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Thompson RG, Moulin CJA, Hayre S, Jones RW. Music enhances category fluency in healthy older adults and Alzheimer’s disease patients. Exp Aging Res 2005; 31: 91–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Baddeley A. Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1986.Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Sharps MJ, Pollitt BK. Category superiority effects and the processing of auditory images. J Gen Psychol 1998; 125:109–16.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Janata P, Tillmann B, Bharucha JJ. Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosc 2002; 2:121–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Slevc LR, Miyake A. Individual differences in second-language proficiency: does musical ability matter? Psychol Sci 2006; 17: 675–81.Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Ho YC, Cheung, MC, Chan AS. Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychol 2003; 17: 439–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Williamon A, Egner T. Memory structures for encoding and retrieving a piece of music: an ERP investigation. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2004; 22: 36–44.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Henry JD, Crawford JR, Phillips LH. Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer type: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2004; 42: 1212–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Hirokawa E. Effects of music listening and relaxation instructions on arousal changes and the working memory task in older adults. J Music Ther 2004; 41:107–27.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189–98.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Mondini S, Mapelli D, Vestri A, Bisiacchi PS. Esame Neuropsicologico Breve. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore, 2003.Google Scholar
  32. 32.
    Daee S, Wilding JM. Effects of high intensity white noise on short-term memory for position in a list and sequence. Brit J Psychol 1977; 68: 335–49.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Pichora-Fuller MK, Schneider BA, Daneman M. How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. J Acoust Soc Am. 1995; 97: 593–608.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Cornoldi C, Vecchi T. Visuo-spatial working memory and individual differences. Hove: Psychology Press, 2003.Google Scholar
  35. 35.
    Steele KM, Ball TN, Runk R. Listening to Mozart does not enhance backwards digit span performance. Percept Mot Skills 1997; 84:1179–84.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Internal Publishing Switzerland 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nicola Mammarella
    • 1
  • Beth Fairfield
    • 1
  • Cesare Cornoldi
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Biomedical SciencesUniversity of Chieti “G.D’Annunzio” and Università Telematica, “L. Da Vinci”Chieti
  2. 2.Department of General PsychologyUniversity of PadovaPadovaItaly
  3. 3.Dipartimento di Scienze Biomediche, Facoltà di PsicologiaUniversità degli Studi “G. D’Annunzio”ChietiItaly

Personalised recommendations