Skip to main content
Log in

Children’s development of a theory of mind

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A critical review of the literature on the theory of mind is presented. Consistent with the “early onset” view, it is suggested that important precursors of a theory of mind are found much earlier than the age of 4. Research on emotional development and intentional communication is reviewed to suggest that some rudimentary understanding of other people’s minds occurs before the age of 2. Later, 3-year-olds’ engagement in pretense and deception demonstrates a more sophisticated understanding of other people’s mental states. Limitations of the false belief task for determining the acquisition of a theory of mind will be discussed with reference to findings in the adult literature.

Résumé

Les auteurs présentent une revue critique de la littérature concernant la théorie de l’esprit. En accord avec le point de vue “early oneset”, il apparaît que d’importants précurseurs de la théorie de l’esprit s’y sont intéressé bien avant l’âge de quatre ans. Les recherches sur le développement émotionnel et la communication intentionnelle suggèrent que certaines compréhensions rudimentaires du mental des autres s’observent avant l’âge de deux ans. Plus tard, à trois ans, les comportements de simulacre et de déceptions plaident en faveur d’une compréhension plus sophistiquée des états mentaux des autres. A partir des résultats provenant de la littérature sur les adultes, les auteurs discutent ensuite des limites inhérentes à la tâche de la fausse croyance pour étudier l’acquisition d’une théorie de l’esprit.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alessandri, S.M., Sullivan, M.W., & Lewis, M. (1990). Violation of expectancy and frustration in early infancy.Developmental Psychology, 26, 738–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J.W., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Theoretical explanations of children’s understanding of the mind.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 7–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1979). Intentions, conventions and symbols. In E. Bates, L. Benigni, I. Bretherton, L. Camaioni, & V. Volterra (Eds.),The emergence of symbols (pp. 33–42). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H.M. (1995).Children talk about the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Benelli, B., Carelli, G., & Arnold, S. (1995). Effetti del tipo di relazione interpersonale sul decentramento e sulla competenza metacognitiva: La teoria della mente in gemelli, fratelli e figli unici.Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 22, 107–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. (1970).Language development: Form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I. (1991). Intentional communication and the development of an understanding of mind. In D. Frye & C. Moore (Eds.),Children’s theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 49–75). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind.Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (1994). Theory of mind and the facts of embodiment. In C. Lewis & P Mitchell (Eds.),Children’s early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 115–132). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M. (1987). The Othello effect: Essay on the Emergence and Eclipse of skeptical doubt.Human Development, 30, 1334–1351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M.J., Fritz, A.S., & Hala, S.M. (1989). Small scale deceit: Deceit as a marker of 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds early theories of mind.Child Development, 60, 1263–1277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H., & Chase, W.G. (1972). On the processes of comparing sentences against pictures.Cognitive Psychologist, 3, 472–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Dale, N. (1984). 1 a daddy: 2-year-olds’ collaboration in joint pretend with sibling and with mother. In I. Bretherton (Ed.),Symbolic play (pp. 131–158). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Levenson, R.W., & Friesen, W.V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions.Science, 221, 1208–1210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L., & Maccoby, N. (1964). On resistance to persuasive communications.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 359–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischoff, B. (1977). Perceived informativeness of facts.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 349–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H., Flavell, E.R., & Green, F.L. (1987). Young children’s knowledge about the apparent-real and pretend-real distinctions.Developmental Psychology, 23, 816–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H., Green, F.L., & Flavell, E.R. (1990). Developmental changes in young children’s knowledge about the mind.Cognitive Development, 5, 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J.A. (1987).Psychosemantics: The problem of meaning in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerow, L., Taylor, M., & Moses, L. (under review).Children’s understanding of pretense as based on mental representation. Unpublished manuscript, University of Oregon.

  • Gilbert, D.T. (1991). How mental systems believe.American Psychologist, 46, 107–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Astington, J.W. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction.Child Development, 59, 26–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Slaughter, V. (1991). Young children’s understanding of changes in their mental states.Child-Development, 62, 98–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P.B. (1965). Grammatical transformations and speed of understanding.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 107–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hala, S., Chandler, M., & Fritz, A.S. (1991). Fledgling theories of mind: Deception as a marker of three-year-olds’ understanding of false belief.Child Development, 63, 83–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P.L. (1989).Children and emotion: The development of psychological understanding. Oxford, England: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P.L., Brown, E., Marriot, C., Whittall, S., & Harmer, S. (1991). Monsters, ghosts, and witches: Testing the limits of the fantasy-reality distinction in young children.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 105–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P.H., Kavanaugh, R.D., & Meredith, M.C. (1994). Young children’s comprehension of pretend episodes: The integration of successive actions.Child Development, 65, 16–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958).The Psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hogrefe, G.J., Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1986). Ignorance versus false belief. A developmental lag in attribution of epistemic states.Child Development, 57, 567–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1980). Mental models in cognitive science.Cognitive Science, 4, 71–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1988). How is meaning mentally represented? In U. Eco, M. Santambrogio, & P. Viloli (Eds.),Meaning and mental representations (pp. 99–118). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S.R., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit?Cognition, 40, 203–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A.M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”.Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A.M. (1988). Some implications of pretense for mechanisms underlying the child’s theory of mind. In J. Astingon, P. Harris, & D. Olson (Eds.),Developing theories of mind (pp. 19–46). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., Alessandri, S.M., & Sullivan, M.W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants.Developmental Psychology, 26, 745–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., Stanger, C., & Sullivan, M.W. (1989). Deception in 3-year-olds.Developmental Psychology, 25, 439–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A.S. (1991).Pretend Play: Zone of proximal development or fool’s gold? Unpublished manuscript.

  • Lillard, A.S. (1993). Young children’s conceptualization of pretense: Action or mental representational state?Child Development, 64, 372–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macnamara, J. (1989). Children as common sense psychologists.Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21, 426–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing theories. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.),Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 335–366). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N., & Moore, M.K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates.Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, P., & Lacohee, H. (1991). Children’s early understanding of false belief.Cognition, 39, 107–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moses, L.J., & Chandler, M.J. (1992). Traveler’s guide to children’s theories of mind.Psychological Inquiry, 3, 286–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moses, L., & Flavell, J. (1990). Inferring false beliefs from actions and reactions.Child Development, 61, 929–945.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, D.K., Astington, J.W., & Flavell, J. (1992). Young children’s understanding of the role that sensory experiences play in knowledge acquisition.Child Development, 63, 474–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, D.K., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Young children’s ability to identify the sources of their beliefs.Developmental Psychology, 27, 390–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., Smithe, P.E., & Yuille, J.C. (1968). Imagery versus meaninfulness of nouns in paired-associate learning.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22, 427–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J.A. (1970). A mathematical model for transition in Piaget’s develpmental stages.Acta Psychologica, 32, 301–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R.D. (1980). The development of negation in early childhood language. In D.R. Olson (Ed.),The social foundations of language and thought (pp. 156–186). New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1988). Higher-order beliefs and intentions in children’s understanding of social interactions. In J.W. Astington, P.L., Harris, & D.R. Olson (Eds.),Developing theories of mind (pp. 271–294). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1991).Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1993). The theory of mind deficit in autism: Rethinking the metarepresentation theory. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D.J. Cohen (Eds.),Understanding others minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 112–137). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Leekman, S.R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds difficulty with false belief. The case for a conceptual deficit.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R.E., & Cacioppo, J.T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 19, pp. 123–205). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pillow, B. (1989). Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 116–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 10, pp. 173–220). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., Lepper, M.R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: Biased attribution processes in the debriefing paradigm.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 880–892.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M., Wellman, H.M., & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state.Cognition, 14, 301–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B., Taylor, C., Harris, P.L., & Perner, J. (1991). Early deception and the child’s theory of mind: False trails and genuine markers.Child Development, 62, 468–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stenberg, C.R., Campos, J.J., & Emde, R.N. (1983). The facial expressions of anger in seven-month-old infants.Child. Development, 54, 178–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. (1985).Conceptual perspective-taking: The development of the ability to represent different interpretations of the same information. Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University.

  • Taylor, M. (1988). The development of children’s ability to distinguish what they know from what they see.Child Development, 58, 424–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M., Cartwright, B.S., & Bowden, T. (1991). Perspective taking and theory of mind: Do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers knowledge?Child Development, 62, 1334–1351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M., Esbensen, B.M., & Bennett, R.T. (1994). Children’s understanding of knowledge acquisition: The tendency for children to report that they have always known what they have just learned.Child Development, 65, 1581–1604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1979). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and the acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.),Action, gesture, and symbol (pp. 183–189). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness.Canadian Psychology, 26, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D.M., Coulton, G., & Wenzlaff, R. (1985). The transparency of denial: Briefing in the debriefing paradigm.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 338–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M. (1985). A child’s theory of mind: The development of conceptions of cognition. In S.R. Yussen (Ed.),The growth of reflection in children (pp. 169–206). New York, NY: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M. (1990).The child’s theory of mina. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M. (1993). Early understanding of mind: The normal case. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen, (Eds.),Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 10–39). Oxford, England: Oxford, University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M., & Estes, D. (1986). Early understanding of mental entities: A reexamination of childhood realism.Child Development, 57, 910–923.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M., Harris, P.L., Banerjee, M., & Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion: Evidence from natural language.Cognition and Emotion, 9, 117–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Hartl, M. (1991). Against the cartesian view of mind: Young children’s difficulty with own false beliefs.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 125–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs. Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception.Cognition, 13, 103–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R.B., Morphy, S.T., & Inglehart, M. (1989). Feeling and facial expression: Implications of the vascular theory of emotion.Psychological Review, 96, 395–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cadinu, M.R., Kiesner, J. Children’s development of a theory of mind. Eur J Psychol Educ 15, 93–111 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173169

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173169

Key words

Navigation