Skip to main content
Log in

The role of reading comprehension in word meaning acquisition during reading

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to explore the influence of reading comprehension on the acquisition of word meanings from context and to compare it to the effects of local context characteristics, such as proximity and directness of context clues. The study also examined the effects of prior topic and enabling concept knowledge. Sixth-grade students were pretested on their knowledge of target vocabulary from two expository passages appropriate for their grade and reading level. The students were then familiarized with selected main concepts from one passage and, subsequently, read both experimental passages. Half of the target words appeared in an informative context, while the rest appeared in an less-informative context. A comprehension test and a vocabulary posttest followed the reading of each passage. Hierarchical Regression analyses indicated that reading comprehension level and prior main concept knowledge facilitated vocabulary learning from context. In comparison, the effect of presence or absence of informative context clues was not significant. These findings, underline the need for a reconceptualization of context to take into account the mental representation that readers construct in addition to the printed text that surrounds an unfamiliar word.

Abstract

L'objet de cette ètude est de savoir si la compréhension d'un texte facilite l'acquisition du sens de certains mots qui le composent et d'examiner par comparaison le rôle de la proximité et de la valeur informative d'índices contextuels locaux. Le rôle des connaissances antérieures en vocabulaire et dans le domaine de référence du texte a été également étudié. Les sujets sont des élèves de 6ème année.

Dans une phase de prétest on évalue la connaissance qu'ils ont d'un ensemble de mots-cibles appartenant à deux textes adaptés à leur niveau de lecture et à leur niveau scolaire. Les élèves sont ensuite familiarisés avec les principaux concepts d'un des deux textes et, immédiatement aprés, lisent les deux textes expérimentaux. La moitié des mots-cibles apparaît dans un contexte très informatif, alors que l'autre moitié apparaît dans un contexte moins informatif. Dans la troisième phase on teste la compréhension des textes et la compréhension des mots-cibles. Une analyse de régression hiérarchique met en évidence que la compréhension des textes et les connaissances lexicales antérieures facilitent l'utilisation du contexte dans l'acquisition du sens des mots. Par comparaison, la présence ou l'absence d'índices contextuels informatifs n'a pas d'effet significatif. Ces résultas soulignent la nécessité d'une reconceptualisation du contexte prenant en compte la représentation mentale construite par les lecteurs, au delà du texte imprimé qui entourne un mot non familier.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Albrecht, J.E., & O'Brien, E.J. (1991). Effects of centrality on retrieval of text-based concepts.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 932–939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R.C. (1990). Inferences about word meanings. In A.C. Graesser & G.H. Bower (Eds.),Psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 25. Inferences and text comprehension (pp. 1–16). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R.C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J.T. Guthrie (Ed.),Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77–117). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R.C., & Nagy, W.E. (1989).Word meanings (Tech. Rep. No. 485). Urbana-Champaign: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R.C., & Pearson, P.D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in reading comprehension. In P.D. Pearson (Ed.),Handbook of reading research (pp. 255–292). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D.P. (1960). The use of advance organizers in the learning and retention of meaningful verbal material.Journal of Educational Psychology, 51, 267–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D.P. (1978). In defense of advance organizers: A reply to critics.Review of Educational Research, 48, 251–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beelitz, P. (1988). Desert traders: The Tuareg.Faces: The Magazine about People, 4, 8–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J.D., Barclay, J.R., & Franks, J.J. (1972). Sentence memory: A constructive versus interpretive approach.Cognitive Psychology, 3, 193–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J.D., & McCarrell, N.S. (1974). A sketch of a cognitive approach to comprehension. In W. Weiner & D. Palermo (Eds.),Cognition and the symbolic process (pp. 189–229). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W.F. (1987). Schemas versus mental models in human memory. In P. Morris (Ed.),Modeling cognition (pp. 187–197). Chichester, MA: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carnine, D., Kameenui, E., & Coyle, G. (1984). Utilization of contextual information in determining the meanings of unfamiliar words.Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 188–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971).Word frequency book. New York: American Heritage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M. (1987). Vocabulary testing and vocabulary instruction. In M. McKeown & M.E. Curtis (Eds.)The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 37–51). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diakidoy, I.N., & Anderson, R.C. (1991).The role of contextual information in word meaning acquisition during normal reading (Tech. Rep. No. 531) Urbana-Champaign: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dooling, D.J., & Christiaansen, R.E. (1977). Episodic and semantic aspects of memory for prose.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 428–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drum, P.A., & Konopak, B.C. (1987). Learning word meanings from written context. In M. McKeown & M.E. Curtis (Eds.),The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 73–87). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freebody, P., & Anderson, R.C. (1983). Effects of vocabulary difficulty, text cohesion, and schema availability on reading comprehension.Reading Research Quarterly, 18, 277–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golin, S., Sweeney, P.D., & Shaeffer, D.E. (1981). The causality of causal attributions in depression: A cross-lagged panel correlational analysis.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 14–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graves, M.F. (1986). Vocabulary learning and instruction. In E.Z. Rothkopf & L.C. Ehri (Eds.),Review of research in education: Vol. 13 (pp. 49–51). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graves, M.F., & Buikema, J.L. (1990, April).Effects of teaching context cues on junior high school students' ability to infer meanings of unknown words. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Boston, MA.

  • Herman, P., & Weaver, C.R. (1988, December).Contextual strategies for learning word meanings: Middle grade students look in and look around. Paper presented at the National Reading Conference, Tucson, AZ.

  • Jenkins, J., & Dixon, R. (1983). Vocabulary learning.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 237–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J., Stein, M., & Wysocki, K. (1984). Learning vocabulary through reading.American Educational Research Journal, 21, 767–788.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M.A., & Carpenter, P.A. (1987).Psychology of reading and language comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, L. (1987). Silk: The caterpillar thread.Faces: The Magazine about People, 4, 24–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D.A. (1974). Cross-lagged panel correlation: A test of spuriousness.Psychological Bulletin, 82, 887–903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D.A., & Harakiewicz, J.M. (1979). Cross-lagged panel correlation: Practice and promise.Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 372–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1986). Learning from text.Cognition and Instruction, 3, 87–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konopak, B.C. (1988). Eighth-grader's vocabulary learning from inconsiderate and considerate text.Reading Research and Instruction, 27, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1988). Contextually relevant aspects of meaning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 331–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mezynski, K. (1983). Issues concerning the acquisition of knowledge: Effects of vocabulary training on reading comprehension.Review of Educational Research, 53, 253–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., & Gildea, P. (1987). How children learn words.Scientific American, 257, 94–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W.E. (1988).Teaching vocabulary to improve reading comprehension. Urbana, IL: NCTE, IRA, & ERJC/RCS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W.E., & Anderson, R.C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English?Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W.E., Anderson, R.C., & Herman, P. (1987). Learning word meanings from context during normal reading.American Educational Research Journal, 24, 237–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W.E., & Herman, P. (1987). Breadth and depth of vocabulary knowledge: Implications for acquisition and instruction. In M. McKeown & M.E. Curtis (Eds.),The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 19–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W.E., Herman, P., & Anderson, R.C. (1985). Learning words from context.Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 233–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nist, S.L., & Olejnik, S. (1995). The role of context and dictionary definitions on varying levels of word knowledge.Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 172–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Omanson, R.C., Warren, W.H., & Trabasso, T. (1978). Goals, inferential comprehension, and recall of stories by children.Discourse Processes, 1, 323–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, P.D., & Gallagher, M. (1983). The instruction of reading comprehension.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 317–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, P.D., & Johnson, D.D. (1978).Teaching reading comprehension. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogosa, D. (1980). A critique of cross-lagged correlation.Psychological Bulletin, 88, 245–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E., & Ortony, A. (1977). The representation of knowledge in memory. In R.C. Anderson, R.J. Spiro, & W.E. Montague (Eds.),Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge (pp. 99–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scha, R.J.H., Bruce, B.C., & Polanyi, L. (1986). Discourse understanding (Tech. Rep. No. 311). Urbana-Champaign: University of Illinois, Center for the Study of Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatz, E., & Baldwin, R.S. (1986). Context clues are unreliable predictors of word meanings.Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 439–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Anderson, R.C., & Zhang, H. (1995). Incidental learning of word meanings while reading: A Chinese and American cross-cultural study.Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 76–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonsen, S., & Singer, H. (1992). Improving reading instruction in the content areas. In S.J. Samuels & A.E. Farstrup (Eds.),What research has to say about reading instruction (pp. 200–219). International Reading Association.

  • Spilich, G.J., Vesonder, G.T., Chiesi, H.L., & Voss, J.F. (1979). Text processing of domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 275–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stallman, A., Pearson, P.D., Nagy, W.E., & Garcia, J. (1990, December).Alternative approaches to vocabulary assessment. Paper presented at the National Reading Conference, San Antonio, TX.

  • Stanovich, K. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J. (1987). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In M. McKeown & M.E. Curtis (Eds.),The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 89–105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J., & Powell, J. (1983). Comprehending verbal comprehension.American Psychologist, 38, 878–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • vanDaalen-Kapteijns, M., & Elshout-Mohr, M. (1981). The acquisition of word meanings as a cognitive learning process.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 386–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zechmeister, E.B., Chronis, A.M., Cull, W.L., D'Anna, C.A., & Healy, N.A. (1995). Growth of a functionally important lexicon.Journal of Reading Behavior, 27, 201–212.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was completed at the Center for the Study of Reading, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, U.S.A. I thank Richard C. Anderson, Bill Nagy, David Pearson, and David Zola for their insightful comments and reviews.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Diakidoy, IA.N. The role of reading comprehension in word meaning acquisition during reading. Eur J Psychol Educ 13, 131–154 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173086

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173086

Key words

Navigation