Skip to main content
Log in

Metacognitive knowledge in primary grades: A longitudinal study

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this longitudinal study was to explore the development of metacognitive knowledge in primary grade children (6–9 years, n=196) from pre-school to third grade. Knowledge about cognitive processes was obtained from children’s oral explanations. The results indicated that the average metacognitive knowledge of the primary grade children developed significantly during the first three school years. The children knew much about simple factors and strategies influencing cognitive activities, but after the first three school years, more complex strategies still remained unfamiliar to them. Moreover, the content analyses of children’s oral explanations revealed interesting differences in children’s metacognitive thinking. There appeared to be two groups of children with either non-developing or developing explanations. In addition, the children understood mental processing better in memory and learning activities, whereas comprehension processes were largely unknown to them.

Résumé

L’objectif de cette étude longitudinale a été d’examiner le développement de la connaissance métacognitive chez 196 enfants scolarisés âgés de 6 à 9 ans (correspondant en Finlande aux grandes classes de la maternelle ou enseignement pré-scolaire et aux trois premières classes d’école primaire). On a obtenu les données sur les processus cognitifs des enfants à partir de leurs explications orales. Les résultats indiquent qu’en moyenne, la connaissance métacognitive de ces enfants s’est considérablement développée pendant les trois premières années de leur scolarité (de 7 à 9 ans). Les enfants connaissent en effet beaucoup sur des facteurs simples et des stratégies qui influencent les activités cognitives, mais après ces trois ans de scolarité, ils ne se familiarisent pas avec des stratégies plus complexes. En outre, l’analyse du contenu des explications orales des enfants montre des différences intéressantes quant à leur réflexion métacognitive. Deux groupes d’enfants se distinguent alors: L’un avec des explications en cours de développement et l’autre avec des explications demeurées inchangées. Enfin, les enfants comprennent mieux les processus mentaux quand ceux-ci sont liés aux activités de mémorisation et d’apprentissage, tandis que les processus de compréhension leur sont plus ou moins inconnus.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alexander, J.M., Carr, M., & Schwanenflugel, P.J. (1995). Development of metacognition in gifted children: Directions for future research.Developmental Review, 15, 1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Estes, D. (1996). Individual differences in children’s developing theory of mind and implications for metacognition.Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 281–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boekarts, M. (1996). Self-regulated learning at the junction of cognition and motivation.European Psychologist, 1, 100–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boekarts, M. (1997). Self-regulated learning: A new concept embraced by researchers, policy makers, educators, teachers, and students.Learning and Instruction, 7, 161–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boekarts, M. (1999). Metacognitive experiences and motivational state as aspects of self-awareness: Review and discussion.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14, 571–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkowski, J.G. (1985). Signs of intelligence: Strategy generalisation and metacognition. In S.R. Yussen (Ed.),The growth of reflection in children (pp. 105–144). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkowski, J.G., & Kurtz, B.E. (1987). Metacognition and cognitive control. In J.G. Borkowski & J.D. Day (Eds.),Cognition in special children: Comparative approaches to retardation, learning disabilities, and giftedness (pp. 123–152). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkowski, J.G., & Muthukrishna, N. (1992). Moving metacognition in to the classroom: “Working models” and effective strategy teaching. In M. Pressley, K. Harris, & J. Guthrie (Eds.),Promoting academic competence and literacy in schools (pp. 478–501). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkowski, J.G., & Muthukrishna, N. (1995). Learning Environments and Skill Generalisation: How contexts facilitate regulatory processes and efficacy beliefs. In F.E. Weinert & Schneider (Eds.),Memory performance and competencies; Issues in growth and development (pp. 283–300). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkowski, J.G., Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1989). The challenges of teaching good information processing to learning disabled students.International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 36, 169–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. (1980). Metacognitive development and reading. In R.J. Spiro, B.C. Bruce, & W.F. Brewer (Eds.),The theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 453–479), Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and other more mysterious mechanisms. In F. Weinert & R. Kluwe (Eds.),Metacognition, motivation and understanding (pp. 65–116). Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A.L. (1994). The Advancement of Learning.Educational Researcher, 23, 4–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J.S. (1990).Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campione, J.C. (1987). Metacognitive components of instructional research with problem learners. In F.E. Weinert & R.H. Kluwe (Eds.),Metacognition, motivation and understanding (pp. 117–140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornoldi, C. (1990). Metacognitive control processes and memory deficits in poor comprehenders.Learning Disability Quarterly, 13, 245–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, G.G. (1993). Teachers’ progress toward becoming expert strategy teachers.The Elementary School Journal, 94, 109–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efklides, A., Samara, A., & Petropoulou, M. (1999). Feeling of difficulty: An aspect of monitoring that influences control.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14, 461–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry.American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H. (1987). Speculations about the nature and development of metacognition. In F.E. Weinert, & R.H. Kluwe (Eds.),Metacognition, motivation and understanding (pp. 21–29). Hillsdale, New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H. (1988). The development of children’s knowledge about the mind. In J.W. Astington, P.L. Harris, & R.O. Olson (Eds.),Developing theories of mind (pp. 244–267). Cambridge: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H., & Wellman, H.M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. Kail & J. Hagen (Eds.),Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition. Hilldale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J.H., Green, F.L., & Flavell, E.R. (1995). Young children’s knowledge about thinking.Monographs of the society for research in child development, 60.

  • Forrest-Pressley, D.L., & Waller, T.G. (1984).Cognition, metacognition and reading. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner, R., & Alexander, P.A. (1989). Metacognition: Answered and unanswered questions.Educational Psychologist, 24, 143–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaskins, I.W. (1994). Classroom applications of cognitive science: Teaching poor reader show to learn, think and problem solve. In K. McGilly (Eds.),Classroom Lessons: Integrating Cognitive Theory and Classroom Practice (pp. 129–154), Cambridge, Massachusetts: A Bradford Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, D.J. (1998). Definitions and empirical foundations. In D.J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & C. Graesser (Eds.),Metacognition in Educational Theory and Practice (pp. 1–23). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, K.R., & Graham, S. (1996). Constructivism and students with special needs: Issues in the classroom.Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 11, 134–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreutzer, M.A., Leonard, S.C., & Flavell, J.H. (1975). An interview study of children’s knowledge about memory.Society for research in child development, 40.

  • Lovett, S.B., & Flavell, J.H. (1990). Understanding and remembering: Children’s knowledge about the differential effects of strategy and task variables on comprehension and memorisation.Child Development, 61, 1842–1858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D.R., & Astington, J.W. (1993). Thinking about thinking: Learning how to take statements and hold beliefs.Educational Psychologist, 28, 7–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S.G., & Byrnes, J.P. (1989). The constructivist approach to self-regulation and learning in the classroom. In B.J. Zimmerman & D.H. Schunk (Eds.),Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theory, research and practice (pp. 169–200). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S.G., & Winograd, P. (1990). How metacognition can promote academic learning and instruction. In B.F. Jones & L. Idol (Eds.),Dimensions of thinking and cognitive instruction (pp. 15–51). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D.N., Simmons, R., & Tishman, S. (1990). Teaching cognitive and metacognitive strategies.Journal of Structural Learning, 10, 285–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Davies, G. (1991). Understanding the mind as an active information processor.Cognition, 39, 51–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinard, A. (1992). Metaconsciousness and metacognition.Canadian Psychology, 33, 27–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M. (1995a). More about development of self-regulation: Complex, long-term, and thoroughly social.Educational Psychologist, 30, 207–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M. (1995b). What is intellectual development about in the 1990’s? Good information processing. In F.E. Weinert & W. Schneider (Eds.),Memory performance and competencies: Issues in growth and development (pp. 375–404). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., Hogan, K., Wharton-McDonald, R., Mistretta, J., & Ettenberger, S. (1996). The challenges of instructional scaffolding: The challenges of instruction that supports student thinking.Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 11, 138–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, D.W. (1989). Pre-schoolers’ use of metacognitive knowledge and strategies in self-selected literacy events.National Reading Conference Yearbook, 38, 65–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1998).Introduction. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 13, 3–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Sodian, B. (1991). A Longitudinal study of young children’s memory behaviour in a sort-recall task.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 14–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Weinert, F.E. (1995). Memory development during early and middle childhood: Findings from the Munich Longitudinal Study (LOGIC). In F.E. Weinert & W. Schneider (Eds.),Memory performance and competencies; Issues in growth and development (pp. 263–279). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Moshman, D. (1995). Metacognitive theories.Educational Psychology Review, 7, 351–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. (1995). Inherent Details of Self-Regulated Learning Include Student Perceptions.Educational Psychologist, 30, 213–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P.J., Fabricius, W.V., & Alexander, J. (1994). Developing theories of mind: Understanding concepts and relations between mental activities.Child Development, 65, 1546–1563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwanenflugel, P.J., Stevens, P.M., & Carr, M. (1997). Metacognitive knowledge of gifted and non-identified children in early elementary school.Gifred Child Quarterly, 41, 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, P.R.J. (1993). Constructive learning: The role of the learner. In T. Duffy, J. Lowyck, & D.H. Jonassen (Eds.),Designing environments for constructive learning, Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vauras, M., Kinnunen, R., & Rauhanummi, T. (1999). The role of metacognition in the context of integrated strategy intervention.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14, 555–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vauras, M., Rauhanummi, T., Kinnunen, R., & Lepola, J. (1999). Motivational vulnerability as a challenge for educational interventions.International Journal of Educational Research, 31, 515–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Wright, J. (1992). Reflections on reflection.Learning and Instruction, 2, 59–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M. (1977). Pre-schoolers’ understanding of memory relevant variables.Child Development, 48, 1720–1723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M. (1978). Knowledge of the interaction of memory variables: A developmental study of metamemory.Developmental Psychology, 14, 24–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M. (1985). The child’s theory of mind. The development of conceptions of cognition. In S.R. Yussen (Ed.),The growth of reflection. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M. (1988). First steps in the child’s theorising about the mind. In J.W. Astington, P.L. Harris, & D.R. Olson (Eds.),Developing theories of mind. Cambridge: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H.M., & Hickling, A.K. (1994). The mind’s “I”: Children’s conception of the mind as an active agent.Child Development, 65, 1564–1580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J.V. (1985).Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind. London: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volet, S. (1997). Process-oriented instruction: A discussion.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 10, 449–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, B.Y.L. (1988). An instructional model for intervention research in learning disabilities.Learning Disabilities Research, 4, 5–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong, B. (1991). The relevance of metacognition to learning disabilities. In B. Wong (Ed.),Learning about learning disabilities (pp. 231–258). California: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1978).Mind in society. The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. (1995). Self-regulation involves more than metacognition: A social cognitive perspective.Educational Psychologist, 30, 217–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tiina Annevirta.

Additional information

This Research was supported by Grant No. 1071265 (1991-30.6.1994) and Grant No. 4131 (1.7.1994–1996) from the Council for Social Sciences Research, the Academy of Finland, to the second author and Pekka Niemi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Annevirta, T., Vauras, M. Metacognitive knowledge in primary grades: A longitudinal study. Eur J Psychol Educ 16, 257–282 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173029

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173029

Key words

Navigation