Abstract
Unpopular children are known to have poor communication skills and experience difficulty in collaborative situations. This study investigated whether pairing unpopular, 5 to 6 year-old, children with a more popular peer would promote more effective collaboration. The study also investigated differences in popular and unpopular children’s verbal and non-verbal communication. Thirty-six girls and 36 boys were placed in one of 12 popular, 12 unpopular or 12 mixed pairs. There were no mixed gender pairs. Children were filmed playing a collaborative game. Collaboration in popular pairs was more successful and less disputational than in unpopular pairs. Boys in unpopular pairs broke the rules of the game more often, argued more and did not monitoring their partners’ facial expressions effectively. With popular partners they argued less, were more likely to elaborate disagreements, looked at their partner for longer, smiled more and were more likely to offer him a small toy. Unpopular girls’ interactions were not markedly disruptive but they clearly benefited from being paired with a child with good communication skills. Popular girls modified their behaviour to take into account an unpopular partner’s need for support. These findings suggest that pairing popular and unpopular children may be a useful classroom organisation strategy.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
References
Azmitia, M. (1988). Peer interaction and problem-solving: When are two heads better than one?Child Development, 59, 87–96.
Azmitia, M. (1996). Peer interactive minds: Developmental, theoretical and methodological issues. In P. Baltes & U. Staudinger (Eds.),Interactive minds: Life-span perspectives on the social foundations of cognition (pp. 133–162). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Barron, A., & Foot, H. (1991). Peer tutoring and tutor training.Educational Research, 33, 174–185.
Black, B. (1992). Negotiating social pretend play: Communicating differences related to social status and sex.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 212–232.
Black, B., & Hazen, N.L. (1990). Social status and patterns of communication in acquainted and unacquainted preschool children.Developmental Psychology, 26, 379–387.
Black, B., & Logan, A. (1995). Links between communication patterns in mother-child, father-child, and child-peer interactions and children’s social status.Child Development, 66, 255–271.
Blaye, A., Light, P., Joiner R., & Sheldon, S. (1991). Collaboration as a facilitator of planning and problem-solving on a computer based task.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 471–483.
Coie, J.D. (Dodge, K.A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and types of social status: A cross-age perspective.Developmental Psychology, 18, 557–570.
Daniels-Bierness, T. (1989). Measuring peer status in boys and girls: A problem of apples and oranges? In B. Schneider, G. Attili, J. Nadel, & R. Weissberg (Eds.),Social competence in the developmental perspective (pp. 107–120). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Denham, S.A., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E.A., & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioural predictors of preschool peer ratings.Child Development, 61, 1145–1152.
Durkin, K. (1995).Developmental social psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.
Hartup, W. (1996). Cooperation, close relationships, and cognitive development. In W.M. Bukowski, A.F. Newcomb, & W.W. Hartup (Eds.),The company they keep: Friendships and their developmental significance (pp. 213–237). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Izard, C. (1995).MAX: The maximally discriminative facial movement coding system (revised). Newark: University of Delaware Instructional Resources Center.
Kemple, K., Speranza, H., & Hazen, N. (1992). Cohesive discourse and peer acceptance: Longitudinal relations in the preschool years.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 364–381.
Kruger, A. (1993). Peer collaboration: Conflict, cooperation or both?Social Development, 2, 165–182.
Kutnick, P. (1997). Computer-based problem-solving: The effects of group composition and social skills on a cognitive, joint action task.Educational Research, 39, 135–147.
Kutnick, P., & Rogers, C. (Eds.). (1994).Groups in Schools. London: Cassell.
Light, P., & Glachan, M. (1985). Facilitation of individual problem solving through peer interaction.Educational Psychology, 5, 217–224.
Littleton, K., & Hakkinen, P. (1999). Learning together: Understanding the processes of computer-based collaborative learning. In P. Dillenbourg (Ed.),Collaborative Learning: Cognitive and Computational Approaches (pp. 20–30). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Markell, R., & Asner, S. (1984). Children’s interactions in Dyads: Interpersonal influence and sociometric status.Child Development, 55, 1412–1424.
Murphy, S. (1999).Young children’s behaviour in interactive tasks: The effects of popularity on communication and task performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Open University, UK.
Newcombe, A., & Bukowski, W. (1983). Social impact and social preference as determinants of children’s peer group status.Developmental Psychology, 19, 856–867.
Nowicki, S. Jr, & Duke, M.P. (1992). The association of children’s nonverbal decoding abilities with their popularity, locus of control and academic acheivement.Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153, 385–393.
Philippot, P., & Feldman, R.S. (1990). Age and social competence in preschooler’s decoding of facial expression.British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 43–54.
Rabiner, D.L., & Gordon, L.V. (1992). The coordination of conflicting social goals: Differences between rejected and non-rejected boys.Child Development, 63, 1344–1350.
Underwood, M. (1993). The relation between social competence and children’s responses to anger-provoking behaviors. In M.K. Underwood & A.L. Zakriski (Eds.),Affective components of social competence: Children’s abilities to send and receive emotional cues. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. New Orleans, LA.
Walden, T.A., & Field, T.M. (1990). Preschool children’s social competence and production and discrimination of affective expression.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 65–76.
Wood, D., Wood, H., Ainsworth, S., & O’Malley, C. (1995). On becoming a tutor: Towards an ontogenetic model.Cognition and Instruction, 13, 565–581
Author information
Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This study was carried out as part of the first author’s PhD dissertation and was supported by Award No. R00429434236 from the Economic and Social Research Council, UK. We would like to express our gratitude to the staff and children of first schools in Hemel Hempstead, without their kindness and co-operation this study would not have been possible.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Murphy, S., Faulkner, D. Learning to collaborate: Can young children develop better communication strategies through collaboration with a more popular peer. Eur J Psychol Educ 15, 389–404 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172983
Received:
Issue Date:
Key words
- Children
- Collaborative Learning
- Gender
- Popularity