Skip to main content
Log in

Description vs explanation and academic evaluation

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

160 secondary school teachers were asked to do attributions for students they knew. The 2x2x2 design incorporated three independent variables: success vs failure, average vs extreme, and explanation vs description. In addition, teachers were asked to express their interest in the student and to indicate their willingness to be interviewed. The main hypothesis was that the effect of categories (success vs failure and of average vs extreme) on the attributions would be stronger in the case of explanation than in the case of description. The findings support predictions. The findings support these assumptions: the effect of success vs failure on the number of attributions and willingness to be interviewed, and the effect of average vs extreme on interest are stronger in the explanation condition than in the descriptive condition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Antaki, C. (Ed.) (1981).The Psychology of ordinary explanations of social behaviour. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antaki, C., & Fielding, G. (1981). Research on ordinary explanations. In C. Antaki. (Ed.),The Psychology of ordinary explanations of social behaviour (pp. 27–56).London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antaki, C. H. (1985). Ordinary explanation in conversation: causal structure and their defence.European Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 213–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauvois, J.L. (1984). La psychologie quotidienne. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, A. R. (1978). Causes and resons in attribution theory: a conceptual critique.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1311–1321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, A. R. (1979). On the relationship between causes and reasons.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1458–1461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, P., & Novick, R. (1990). A probabilistic contrast model of causal induction.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 545–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deschamps, J. C. (1982).L’échec scolaire. Modèle d’élève ou élèves modèles. Lausanne: Pierre-Marcel Favre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, B. L. (1976). Differential social perception and attribution of intergroup violence: testing the lower limits of stereotyping of blacks.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 590–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fincham, F. D., & Jaspars, J. M. (1980). Attribution of responsability: from man the scientist to man the lawyer. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 81–138). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1984).Social cognition. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilly, M. (1980).Maïtre-élève: rôles institutionnels et représentations. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, P. (1987). Rôle institutionnel et attributions de la réusite et de l’échec.L’orientation scolaire et professionnelle, 16, 307–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, P. (1992c). Les effets de l’extrêmisation de la réussite et de l’échec des élèves sur les attributions des enseignants.International Journal of Psychology, 27, 280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, P. (1992b). Les attributions de la réussite et de l’échec chez les enseignants: justification pédagogique ou justification de l’évaluation?Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 2, 73–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, P. (1992a).Qui est responsable de l’échec scolaire?. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, P. (1994). The attribution of success and failure: the subject/object contrast.European Journal of Pschology of Education, 9, 69–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, V. L. (1978). Who is responsible? Toward a social psychology of responsibility attribution.Social Psychology, 41, 316–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, J. H., Arkin, R., Gleason, J. M., & Johnston, S. (1974). Effect of expected and observed outcome of an action on the differential causal attribution of actor and observer.Journal of Personality, 42, 62–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. (1958).The Psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hewstone, M., Gale, L., & Purkhardt, N. (1990). Intergroup attributions for success and failure: group-serving bias and group-serving causal schemata.Cahiers de psychologie cognitive, 10, 23–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewstone, M. (1989).Causal attribution. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In L. Levine (Ed.),Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 192–238). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and research.Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 171–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. (1975). The endogeneous-exogeneous partition in attribution theory.Psychological Review, 1975,82, 387–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. (1979). Causal explanation, teological explanation: on radical particularism in attribution theory.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1447–1457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1990).La tension essentielle. Tradition et changement dans les sciences. Paris: Gallimard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. (1989). Minding matters, the consequences of mindlessness-mindfulness.Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 137–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Le Poultier, F. (1989). Acquisition de la norme d’internalité et activité évaluative. In in J. L. Beauvois, R. V. Joule, & J. M. Monteil (Eds),Perspectives cognitives et conduites sociales, Vol. 2 (pp. 247–257). Fribourg: Del Val.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, (1974).The cement of the universe: the study of causation. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matalon, B. (1988).Décrire, expliquer, prévoir. Paris: Armand Colin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medway, F. J. (1979). Causal attribution for school related problems.Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 809–819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Medway, F. J., & Cafferty T. P. (1992).School Psychology, a social psychology perspective. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteil, J.M. (1986). Attribution et mobilisation d’une appartenance idéologique, un effet polydoxique.Psychologie Française, 31, 115–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteil, J. M. (1989).Eduquer et former. Grenoble: Presses Universitaires de Grenoble.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteil, J. M. (1993).Soi et le contexte. Paris: Armand Colin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moscovici, S. (1983). Social representations and social explications, from “naive” scientist to scientist. In M. Hewstone (Ed.)Attribution theory: social and functional extensions (pp. 98–125). Oxford: Basie Blackwell Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mugny, G., & Carugati, F. (1985).L’intelligence au pluriel. Fribourg: Del Val.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noizet, N., & Caverni, J. P. (1978).Psychologie de l’évaluation scolaire. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakes, P. J., Turner, J. C., & Haslam, S. A. (1991). Perceiving people as group members: the role of fit in the salience of social categorizations.British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 125–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schopler, J., & Layton, B. D. (1972).Attribution of interpersonnal power and influence. Morristown: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semin, G. R., & Fiedler K. (1989). Relocating attribution phenomena within a language-cognition interface, the case of actors’ and observers’ perspectives.European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 491–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tedeschi, J. T., & Reiss, M. (1981). Verbal strategies in impression management. In C. Antaki (Ed.),The Psychology of ordinary explanations of social behaviour (pp. 271–309). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Der Pligt, J. (1981). Actor’s and observer’s explanations: divergent perspectives or divergent evaluations" In Antaki C. (Ed.).The psychology of ordinary explanations of social behavior, (pp. 97–118). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R.M. (1972). Percieving the causes of success and failure. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds),Attribution, perceiving the causes of the behavior (pp. 95–120). Morristown: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1972). Causal ascriptions and achievement behavior, a conceptual analysis of effort and reanalysis of locus of control.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 239–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences.Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985b). “Spontaneous” causal thinking.Psychological Bulletin, 97, 74–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985a). An attributional theory of achievment motivation and emotion.Psychological Review, 90, 548–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gosling, P. Description vs explanation and academic evaluation. Eur J Psychol Educ 10, 41 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172794

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172794

Key word

Navigation