Skip to main content
Log in

Task-related variation in communication of mothers and their sons with learning disability

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of the present study was to examine whether mother-child communication patterns vary as a function of the type of the task. Groups of learning disabled (LD=30) and normally achieving boys (NLD=30) were videotaped interacting with their mothers in two different tasks. The children were matched for age (8 to 11 year-olds) and for parent’s SES. The results indicated that the teaching task differentiated the groups more than did the story task. Academic character of the teaching task increased mothers’ task involvement in both groups. Mothers of the LD group showed, however, significantly more dominance and expressed less emotionality while teaching their child. Mothers’ interaction partly followed from their children’s behaviour on this task. The children with LD did not cooperate with their mothers and were not emotionally involved in the teaching task as highly as were the normally achieving children. Corresponding features of interaction were not found for the LD group in the story task. Consistency of children’s communication across the tasks was significant in the LD group. The normally achieving boys were more responsive to changes in task conditions showing different behavior as a function of task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aragona, J., & Eyberg, S. (1981). Neglected children: Mothers’ report of child behavior problems and observed verbal behavior.Child Development, 52, 596–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryan, T., Donahue, M., Pearl, R., & Herzog, A. (1984). Conversational interactions between mothers and learning disabled or nondisabled children during a problem solving task.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49, 64–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. (1973). Mother-child interaction in reflective, impulsive, and hyperactive children.Developmental Psychology, 8, 341–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeRenzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1978). Development of a shortened version of the Token Test.Cortex, 14, 41–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diaz, R., Neal, C., & Vachio, A. (1991). Maternal teaching in the zone of proximal development: A comparison of low- and high-risk dyads.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1, 83–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ditton, P., Green, R., & Singer, M. (1987). Communication deviances: A comparison between parents of learning-disabled and normally achieving students.Family Process, 26, 75–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donahue, M., Pearl, R., & Bryan, T. (1980). Learning disabled children’s conversational competence: response to inadequate messages.Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 387–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doyles, D., Cartelli, L., & Doster, J. (1976). Comparison of patterns of mother-child interaction.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 9, 371–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunham, P., & Dunham, F. (1990). Effects of mothers-infant social interactions on infants’ subsequent contingency task performance.Child Development, 61, 785–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, C. (1981).Peabody picture vocabulary test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, R.-J. (1989). “Learning to learn” and the family system: New perspectives on underachievement and learning disorders.Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 15, 187–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, R-J. (1990). Family communication and children’s learning disabilities: Evidence for Cole’s theory of interactivity.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 145–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammill, D., Leigh, J., McNutt, G., & Larsen, S. (1981). A new definition of learning disabilities.Learning Disability Quarterly, 4, 336–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983).The Boston Naming Test (2nd ed.), Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kistner, J., & Torgesen, J. (1987). Motivational and cognitive aspects of learning disabilities. In A. Kazdin & B. Lahey (Eds.),Advances in clinical child psychology (pp. 289–333). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mash, E., & Mercer, B. (1979). A comparison of the behavior of deviant and non-deviant boys while playing alone and interacting with a sibling.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 20, 197–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGillicuddy-DeLisi, B. (1992). Correlates of parental teaching strategies in families of children evidencing normal and atypical development.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 13, 215–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Närhi, V., & Ahonen, T. (1992). How does clinical diagnosis of LD subtypes match with psychometric test evidence: a research plan.NMI-Bulletin, 2, 24–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A., Brody, G., & Sigel, I. (1985). Parents’ teaching strategies with their children: The effects of parental and child status variables.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 6, 509–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasku-Puttonen, H., Lyytinen, P., & Vilkman, E. (1991). Communication between mothers and their normal or developmentally dysphasic children in experimental sessions.Scandinavian Journal of Logopedics and Phoniatrics, 16, 17–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasku-Puttonen, H., Lyytinen, P., Ahonen, T., Poikkeus, A.-M., & Laakso, M.-L. (1994). Communication deviances and clarity among the mothers of normally achieving and learning disabled boys.Family Process, 33, 11–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. (1956).Colored Progressive Matrices Sets A, Ab, B. London: Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Ellis, S., & Gardner, W. (1984). Adjustment of adult-child instruction according to child’s age task.Developmental Psychology, 20, 193–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shneidman, E. (1952).Manual for the Make a Picture Story Method. Projective Techniques Monographs, 2. The Society for Personality Assessment.

  • Sigel, I., Stinson, E., & Flaugher, J. (1991). Socialization of representational competency in the family: The distancing paradigm. In L. Okagaki & R. Sternberg (Eds.),Directors of development. Influences on the development of children’s thinking (pp. 121–144). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinert, Y., Campbell, S., & Kiely, M. (1981). A comparison of maternal and remedial teacher teaching styles with good and poor readers.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 14, 38–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tollison, P., Palmer, D., & Stowe, M. (1987). Mothers’ expectations, interactions, and achievement attributions for their learning disabled or normally achieving sons.The Journal of Special Education, 21, 83–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J., & Rogoff, B. (1984). Editors’ notes. In B. Rogoff & J. Wertsch, (Eds.),Children’s learning in the “zone of proximal development”: New directions for child development (pp. 1–6). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1986). Rapid alternating stimulus (R.A.S.) naming in the developmental, dyslexias: A developmental, process-differentiated account of rate deficits.Brain and Language, 27, 360–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Worden, P., Kee, D., & Ingle, M. (1987) Parental teaching strategies with preschoolers: A comparison of mothers and fathers within different alphabet tasks.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 12, 95–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, S. (1985). A context for hyperactivity. In K. Gadow (Ed.),Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities (Vol. 4, pp. 273–343). Greenwitch: Jai Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lyytinen, P., Rasku-Puttonen, H., Ahonen, T. et al. Task-related variation in communication of mothers and their sons with learning disability. Eur J Psychol Educ 10, 3 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172791

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172791

Key words

Navigation