Skip to main content
Log in

Aandacht, mentaal tempo en executieve controle na traumatisch hersenletsel

  • Artikelen
  • Published:
Neuropraxis

Abstract

Na een traumatisch hersenletsel, een veel voorkomende neurologische aandoening, heeft een groot deel van de patiënten cognitieve klachten, met name op het gebied van aandacht. In de literatuur is er consensus over een vertraagd tempo van informatieverwerking als blijvend restverschijnsel, maar als het gaat om stoornissen in aandacht en executieve functies lopen de bevindingen uiteen. In dit artikel wordt een promotieonderzoek beschreven dat erop gericht was dergelijke stoornissen, eventueel herstel, en de gevolgen ervan voor het dagelijks leven in kaart te brengen. Tevens werden theoretische vragen beantwoord over de onderlinge relatie van de begrippen ‘tempo’, ‘selectieve aandacht’ en ‘executieve controle’, en de meetbaarheid hiervan op testniveau.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Literatuur

  • Adams, J.H., Scott, G., Parker, L.S., Graham, D.I. & Doyle, D. (1980). The contusion index: a quantitative approach to cerebral contusions in head injury. Journal of Neuropathology and applied Neurobiology, 6, 319-324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balen, H.G.G. van (1997). A disability-oriented approach to long-term sequelae following traumatic brain injury. Doctoral dissertation, Catholic University of Nijmegen.

  • Bergner, M., Bobbitt, R.A., Carter, W.B. & Gilson, B.S. (1981). The Sickness Impact Profile: development and final revision of a health status measure. Medical Care, 19, 787-805.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, T.M., & Sautter, S.W. (1993). Route-finding: a measure of everyday executive functioning in the head-injured adult. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7, 171-181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, D.N. (1984). Closed Head Injury: Psychological, Social and Family consequences. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Brooks, D.N., Aughton, M.E., Bond, M., Jones, P. & Rizvi, S. (1980). Cognitive sequelae in relationship to early indices of severity of brain damage after severe blunt head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 43, 529-534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brouwer, W.H. (1985). Limitations of attention after closed head injury. Doctoral dissertation, State University of Groningen, the Netherlands.

  • Brouwer, W.H., Ponds, R.W.H.M., Wolffelaar, P.C. van, & Zomeren, A.H. van (1989). Divided attention 5 to 10 years after closed head injury. Cortex, 25, 219-230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R.A. (1993). The Neuropsychology of Attention. New York and London: Plenum Press.

  • Fasotti, L., & Spikman, J.M. (2002). Cognitive rehabilitation of central executive disorders. In W.H. Brouwer, A.H. van Zomeren, I.J. Berg, J.M. Bouma & E.H.F.de Haan, Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: A cognitive approach. Boom: Amsterdam.

  • Gronwall, D., & Sampson, H. (1974). The Psychological Effects of Concussion. Auckland: Auckland University Press.

  • Hartman, A., Pickering, R.M. & Wilson, B.A. (1992). Is there a central executive deficit after severe head injury? Clinical Rehabilitation, 6, 133-140.

  • James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology, vol 1. New York: Holt & Co.

  • Jennett, B. & Teasdale, G. (1981). Management of head injuries. Contemporary Neurology Series, 20. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis Company.

  • Lezak, M.D. (1982). The problem of assessing executive functions. International Journal of Psychology, 17, 281-297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. London: Tavistock Publications.

  • Miller, E. (1970). Simple and choice reaction time following severe head injury. Cortex, 6, 121-127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minderhoud, J.M., & Zomeren, A.H. van (1984). Traumatische hersenletsels. Utrecht/Antwerpen: Bohn, Scheltema & Holkema.

  • Naalt, J. van der (2000). Mild to moderate head injury: long-term outcome and prognostic factors. Doctoral dissertation, State University of Groningen.

  • Nelson, H.E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe damage. Cortex, 12, 313-324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In R.J. Davidson, G.E.Schwartz, & D.Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (pp. 1-18). New York: Plenum.

  • Ponsford, J., & Kinsella, G. (1992). Attentional deficits following closed head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 822-838.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J.T. (1984). Lapses of attention in everyday life. In R. Parasuraman and R.D.Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention. Orlando: Academic Press.

  • Reitan, R.M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indication of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271-276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, J.T.E. (2000). Clinical and neuropsychological aspects of closed head injury. London: Taylor & Francis.

  • Russell, W.R. (1971). The Traumatic Amnesias. London: Oxford University Press.

  • Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. In D.E. Broadbent, & L. Weiskrantz (Eds), The neuropsychology of cognitive function (pp.199-209). London: The Royal Society.

  • Spikman, J.M. (2001). Attention, mental speed and executive control after closed head injury: deficits, recovery and outcome. Doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen.

  • Spikman, J.M., Deelman, B.G., & van Zomeren, A.H. (2000). Executive functioning, attention and frontal lesions in patiënts with chronic CHI. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 325-338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spikman, J.M., Kiers, H.A.L., Deelman, B.G., & Zomeren, A.H. van (2001). Construct validity of concepts of attention in healthy controls and closed head injured patiënts. Brain and Cognition, 47, 446-460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spikman, J.M., Timmerman, M.E., van Zomeren, A.H., & Deelman, B.G. (1999). Recovery versus retest effects in attention after closed head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 585-605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spikman, J.M., van Zomeren, A.H., & Deelman, B.G. (1996). Deficits of attention after closed-head injury: Slowness only? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 755-767.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoltze, A. (1991). Konstruktion eines neuropsychologischen tests zur messing von planungsfähigkeiten nach frontalhirnschädigung. Diplomarbeit Fachbereich Psychologie Universität Konstanz.

  • Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D.T., Stethem, L.L., Hugenholtz, H., Picton, T., Pivik, J., & Richard, M.T. (1989). Reaction time after head injury: Fatigue, divided and focused attention and consistency of performance. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 52, 742-748.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D.T., Ely, P., Hugenholtz, H., Richard, M.T., LaRochelle, S., Poirier, C.A., & Bell, I. (1985). Subtle neuropsychological deficits in patiënts with good recovery after closed head injury. Neurosurgery, 17, 41-47.

  • Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired unconsciousness. Lancet, 1974, ii:81-83.

  • Teasdale, G., & Mendelow, D. (1984). Pathophysiology of head injuries. In N.Brooks (Ed.), Closed Head Injury: Psychological, Social and Family Consequences (pp. 4-36). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Timmerman, M.E., & Brouwer, W.H. (1999). Slow information processing after very severe closed head injury: impaired access to declarative knowledge and intact application and acquisition of procedural knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 37, 467-478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veltman, J.C., Brouwer, W.H., Zomeren, A.H. van, & Wolffelaar, P.C. van (1996). Central executive aspects of attention in subacute severe and very severe closed head injury patiënts: Planning, inhibition, flexibility and divided attention. Neuropsychology, 10, 1-11.

  • Vilkki, J., Ahola, K., Holst, P., Öhman, J., Servo, A. & Heiskanen, O. (1994). Prediction of psychosocial recovery after head injury with cognitive tests and neurobehavioural ratings. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 325-338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ylvisaker, M. (1998). Traumatic brain injury rehabilitation: children and adolescents. Butterworth Heineman, Boston.

  • Zomeren, A.H. van (1981). Reaction time and attention after closed head injury. Lisse: Swets Publishing.

  • Zomeren, A.H. van, & Brouwer, W.H. (1994). Clinical Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Zomeren, A.H. van, & Burg, W.H. van den (1985). Residual complaints of patiënts two years after severe head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 48, 21-28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zomeren, A.H. van, & Deelman, B.G. (1978). Long term recovery of simple and choice reaction after closed head injury. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 79, 81-90.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Spikman, J. Aandacht, mentaal tempo en executieve controle na traumatisch hersenletsel. NEPR 6, 86–92 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03071023

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03071023

Navigation