The effect of fatigue, sleep deprivation and onerous working hours on the physical and mental wellbeing of pre-registration house officers

Abstract

The potential deleterious effects of doctors’ long and arduous shifts have received relatively scant attention.

This study addressed the effect of a 32 h on-call shift on 16 pre-registration medical house officers in St. James’s Hospital, Dublin.

We assessed 5 psychological parameters (Tension-Anxiety, Depression-Dejection, Vigour-Activity, Fatigue-Inertia and Confusion-Bewilderment) as well as 5 simple tests of alertness and concentration both pre-and post-call. The doctors were randomly assigned to be tested either pre-or post-call.

On average the doctors got 4.5 hours sleep during a 32 h shift. This long shift had an adverse effect on all the psychological parameters (p>0.05) except Depression-Dejection. The total mood disturbance score, which has been shown to correlate well with general psychological well-being, deteriorated significantly after the 32 h shift, p>0.005. Two of the simple tests of alertness and concentration (Trail-making test and Stroop Color-Word test) also showed a significant fall-off in performance with sleep deprivation, p>0.05, although the remaining tests (Delayed Story Recall, Critical Flicker Fusion and Three Minute Grammatical Reasoning Test) were not significantly impaired by the 32 h shift.

This study shows that prolonged periods of duty without sleep adversely affect junior doctors, both in their psychological well-being and in their ability to carry out simple tasks.

References

  1. 1.
    Schweitzer, B. Stress and burnout in junior doctors. S. Afr. Med. J. 1994; 84 (6): 352–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    King, M. B., Cockcroft, A., Gooch, C. Emotional distress in doctors: sources, effects and help sought. J. R. Soc. Med. 1992; 85 (10): 605–8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Junior doctors’ unhappy hours. Br. J. Hosp. Med. 1993; 49 (8): 589.Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Godlee, F. Juniors’ hours: is the end in sight? BMJ. 1992; 305 (6859): 937–40.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    O’Neill, E. Junior doctors’ hours. Flexible partial shifts should be made to work. BMJ. 1995; 311(7015): 1299.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Wong, C. Junior doctors’ hours. Members of junior doctors committee are in cloud cuckoo land. BMJ. 1995; 311 (7005): 632.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    West, S., Stephenson, T. P. Junior doctors’ hours. Long hours are needed. BMJ. 1995; 311 (7012): 1093–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    McCombe, A. Junior doctors’ hours. Wong was right. BMJ. 1995; 311 (7012): 1094.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Asch, D. A., Parker, R. M. The Libby Zion case: one step forward or two steps backward? N. Engl. J. Med. 1988; 318: 771–5.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Hoffman J. Jurors find shared blame in ’84 death. The New York Times. 7th February 1995; B1.Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Orton, D., Gruzelier, J. H. Adverse changes in mood and cognitive performance of house officers after night duty. Br. Med. J. 1989; 298: 21–3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Samkoff, J. S., Jacques, C. H. M. A Review of Studies concerning effects of sleep deprivation and fatigue on residents’ performance. Academic Medicine. 1991; 66: 11; 687–693.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    McNair, D. M, Lorr, M., Droppleman, L. F. Profile of Mood States. Educational and Industrial Testing Service, San Diego, California 92107. 1971.Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Baddely, A. D., Cockburn, J., Wilson, B. The Rivermead behavioural memory test, 1985. Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Hindmarch, I. The effects of psychoactive drugs on psychomotor integration and performance (An introduction to the Leeds Psychomotor Tester), Hoechst Meducation Service.Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Parrott, A. C. Critical Flicker Fusion Thresholds and their Relationship to other Measures of Alertness. Pharmacopsychiat. 1982; 15 (Suppl. 1): 39–43.Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Hindmarch, I. Psychomotor Function and Psychoactive Drugs. Br. J. Pharmac. 1980; 10: 189–209.Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Stone, B. M. Pencil and paper tests - sensitivity to psychotropic drugs. Br. J. Clin. Pharmac. 1984; 18 (supplement): 15s-20s.Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Dyer, F. The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the study of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes. Memory & Cognition. 1973; 1 (2): 106–120.Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Stirling, N. Stroop Interference: an input and output phenomenon. Quarterly Journal of Exp. Psychol. 1979; 31: 121–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Baddely, A. D. A 3 minute reasoning test based on grammatical transformation. Psychol. Sci. 1968; 10 (10): 341–342.Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Motulsky, H. 1995. Intuitive Biostatistics. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Ferguson, C., Shandall, A., Griffith, G. Out-of-hours workload of junior and senior house surgeons in a district general hospital. Ann. R. Coll. Surg. Engl. 1994; 76 (suppl 2): 53–6.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • C. Leonard
    • 1
    • 2
  • N. Fanning
    • 1
  • J. Attwood
    • 1
  • M. Buckley
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of MedicineSt. James’s HospitalDublin 8
  2. 2.Division of Pulmonary and Critical Care MedicineStanford University Medical CenterStanford

Personalised recommendations