Skip to main content
Log in

Vivid recollection as a technique to arouse implicit motive-related affect

  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the links between implicit motives, pleasant experiences, and associated affect. Subjects high in the need for power (n Pow) or intimacy (n Int) recalled a pleasant or neutral personal experience. Consistent with past findings, content analyses revealed that recalled pleasant experiences reflected subjects, respective concerns for power and intimacy. Because the content of the pleasant memories ofn Pow andn Int subjects differed, their vivid recollections were also expected to produce different affective states. Analyses of self-report affect measures administered after the recall task showed thatn Int subjects who recalled a pleasant event expressed more happiness, elation, and friendliness. By contrast,n Pow subjects in the pleasant recall condition reported more excitation and anger. Findings suggest that positive affect inductions involving the vivid recall of idiographic material may lead to different specific affective states based on the content of those experiences and individuals’ capacity to experience and sustain particular affective states.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976).Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W. (1958).Motives in fantasy, action and society. New York: Van Nostrand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakan, D. (1966).The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review.Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. (Ed.). (1991).Emotion and social judgements. Oxford, England: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., & Hesse, F. W. (1994). Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: A review.British Journal of Psychology, 85, 55–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.),A model for personality (pp. 246–276). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.),Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 179–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1989). Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative mood induction procedures.Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1221–1228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., Sinnett, L., & Kasimatis, M. (1988, April).Techniques of laboratory mood induction: A meta-analysis of 100 published studies. Paper presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago.

  • McAdams, D. P. (1982). Experiences of intimacy and power: Relationships between social motives and autobiographical memory.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 292–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (1984). Scoring manual for the intimacy motive.Psychological Documents, 14, 2614.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (1985).Power, intimacy, and the life story: Personological inquiries into identity. Chicago: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (1990).The person. San Diego: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P., Lester, R. M., Brand, P. A., McNamara, W. J., & Lensky, D. B. (1988). Sex and the TAT: Are women more intimate than men? Do men fear intimacy?Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 397–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P., Manfield, E. D., Day, R., & Hoffman, B. J. (1993).Themes of agency and communion in significant autobiographical scenes. Unpublished manuscript, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1975).Power: The inner experience. New York: Irvington.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1980). Motive dispositions: The merits of operant and respondent measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.),Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 10–41). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1981). Is personality consistent? In A. I. Rabin, J. Aronoff, & R. Zucker (Eds.),Further explorations in personality (pp. 86–113). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1985).Human Motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott-Foresman.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1989). Motivational factors in health and disease.American Psychologist, 44, 675–683.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ?Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. P. (Ed.). (1992).Handbook of thematic analysis. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger, E. (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, mood, and mode of thinking.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1460–1469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states.Behavior Research and Therapy, 6, 473–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, J. & McClelland, D. C. (1991). Cognitive versus traditional motivational models. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.),Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 562–597). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G. (1980). Generalization in the effects of a mood induction procedure.Behavior Research and Therapy, 18, 565–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1973).The power motive. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1992). Power motivation revisited. In C. Smith (Ed.),Handbook of thematic analysis (pp. 311–324). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woike, B. A. (1992).A functional utility model of cognitive complexity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Michigan State University, East Lansing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woike, B. A. (1994). The use of differentiation and integration processes: Empirical studies of “separate” and “connected” ways of thinking.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 142–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woike, B. A. (1995). Most memorable experiences: Evidence for a link between implicit and explicit motives and social cognitive processes in everyday life.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68.

  • Woike, B. A., & Aronoff, J. (1992). Antecedents of complex social cognitions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 97–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1985).Manual for the Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist-Revised. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

I am grateful for helpful comments from Alice M. Isen, Dan McAdams, Mike DeBoer, and anonymous reviewers on earlier drafts of this article. The help of Chris Adams, Steve Dean, Brian Downey, Michelle Kallus, Amy Kief, Tom Osier, Camille Tower, and Felicia Wilson in coding the experimental data is also greatly appreciated.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Woike, B.A. Vivid recollection as a technique to arouse implicit motive-related affect. Motiv Emot 18, 335–349 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02856473

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02856473

Keywords

Navigation