Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of three algorithmic representations on critical thinking, laboratory efficiency, and final grade

  • Articles
  • Published:
ECTJ Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Does the way instructions for carrying out elementary operations are presented affect the time required to complete those procedures, student’s critical thinking ability, and student’s final grade? This investigation sought answers by presenting prescriptions — or algorithms — in three ways to university students in an introductory laboratory-chemistry course. Differences in the effects of the three representation modes — flow charts, lists, and standard prose — were complex and changed over the 10 lab sessions. There was no evidence that representation mode affected either critical thinking ability or final grade.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Berlyne, D.Structure and direction in thinking. New York: Wiley, 1965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Lennenberg, E. A study in language and cognition.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1954,49, 452–462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. The course of cognitive growth.American Psychologist, 1964,19, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, F. The new languages. In E. Carpenter and M. McLuhan (Eds.),Explorations in communication. Boston: Beacon Press, 1960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapanis, A. Words, words, words.Human Factors, 1965,7, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. Why can’t leaflets be logical?New Society, 1964,102, 16–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landa, L.Algorithmization in learning and instruction. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Educational Technology Publications, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, B., Horabin, I., & Gane, C.Case studies in the use of algorithms. Oxford: Pergamon, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, B., Horabin, I., & Gane, C.Flow charts, logical trees and algorithms for rules and regulations. London: Her Majesty’s Stationery Office, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, E. F.Design and analysis of experiments in psychology and instruction. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G. Can we affect cognitive skills through visual media? An hypothesis and initial findings.AV Communication Review, 1972,20, 401–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, G., & Glaser, E.Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal test manual. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World, 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P., & Reid, F. Written information: Some alternatives to prose for expressing the outcomes of complex contingencies.Journal of Applied Psychology, 1973,57(2), 160–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The authors are grateful to Martha O. Visscher and Agnes Tenny for their assistance and to Lawson Hughes for suggestions on data analysis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Coscarelli, W.C., Schwen, T.M. Effects of three algorithmic representations on critical thinking, laboratory efficiency, and final grade. Educational Communication and Technology Journal 27, 58–64 (1979). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02765317

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02765317

Keywords

Navigation