Abstract
Effects of smoking on spontaneous bilateral skin conductance were studied as a consequence of interactions with set and setting. Bilateral skin conductance was recorded in six men and six women who smoked and an equal number of nonsmokers on two different days. On the first day, spontaneous electrodermal activity was recorded before and after a 10-minute rest period during which half the smokers smoked a cigarette. On the second day, one week later, spontaneous activity was again recorded before and after a 10-minute rest period during which the remaining smokers smoked a cigarette. Nonsmokers did not smoke at any time. Following smoking there was a significant negative correlation between a preference for visual over nonvisual imagery and spontaneous skin conductance responsivity in the left as compared to the right hand. Correlations were significantly different in smokers and nonsmokers. Smokers showed significantly greater preferences for either visual or nonvisual imagery than nonsmokers. Nonresponding nonsmokers were higher on the psychoticism (P) scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) than responding nonsmokers. Smokers scored higher on P than nonsmokers. Interactions between personality, cognitive style, and the experimental situation, set, and setting were discussed in relation to the arousing effects of nicotine.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
American Psychological Association. (1981) Ethical principles of psychologists.American Psychologist, 36, 633–638.
Annett, M. (1970) A classification of hand preference by association analysis.British Journal of Psychology, 61, 303–321.
Anokhin, P. K. (1974)Biology and Neurophysiology of the Conditioned Reflex and Its Role in Adaptive Behavior. Oxford: Pergammon Press.
Barry, R. J. (1980) Electrodermal responses to emotive and non-emotive words as a function of personality differences in affect level.Biological Psychology, 11, 161–168.
Boyd, G. M., and Maltzman, I. (1984) Effects of cigarette smoking on bilateral skin conductance.Psychophysiology, 21, 334–341.
Drake, R. A. (1984) Lateral asymmetry of personal optimism.Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 497–507.
Eysenck, H. J. (1967)The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975)Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London: Hodder & Staughton.
Farah, M. J., Gazzaniga, M. S., Holtzman, J. D., and Kosslyn, S. M. (1985) A left hemisphere basis for visual mental imagery?Neuropsychologia, 23, 115–118.
Friedman, J., Hovarth, T., and Meares, R. (1974) Tobacco smoking and a “stimulus barrier.”Nature, 248, 455–456.
Gainotti, G. (1983) Laterality of affect: the emotional behavior of right- and left-brain-damaged patients. In M. Myslobodsky (Ed.),Hemisyndromes: Psychology, Neurology, Psychiatry. New York: Academic Press, 175–192.
Gough, H. G. (1960) Theory and measurement of socialization.Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 23–30.
Gray, J. A. (1964)Pavlov’s Typology. Oxford: Pergammon.
Kinsbourne, M. (1970) The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention.Acta Psychologica, 33, 193–201.
Lacroix, J. M., and Comper, P. (1979) Lateralization in the electrodermal system as a function of cognitive/hemispheric manipulations.Psychophysiology, 16, 116–129.
Livanov, M. N. (1977)Spatial Organization of Cerebral Processes. New York: Wiley.
Ludwig, A. M., Wikler, A., and Stark, L. H. (1974) The first drink: psychobiological aspects of craving.Archives of General Psychiatry, 30, 539–547.
Lykken, D. T. (1959) Properties of electrodes used in electrodermal measurements.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 629–634.
Maltzman, I. (1979) Orienting reflexes and conditioning in humans. In H. D. Kimmel, E. H. Van Olst, and J. F., Orlebeke (Eds.),The Orienting Reflex in Humans. New York: Erlbaum, 323–351.
Maltzman, I., Smith, M. J., Kantor, W., and Mandell, M. P. (1971) Effect of stress on habituation of the orienting reflex.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 207–214.
Myslobodsky, M. S. (Ed.) (1983)Hemisyndromes: Psychobiology, Neurology, Psychiatry. New York: Academic Press.
Rusinov, V. S. (1973)The Dominant Focus: Electrophysiological Investigations. New York: Consultants Bureau.
Russell, M. A. H. (1976) Tobacco smoking and nicotine dependence. In R. J. Gibbins, Y. Israel, H. J. Kalant, R. E. Popham, W. Schmidt, and R. G. Smart (Eds.),Research Advances in Alcohol and Drug Problems, Vol. 3. New York: Wiley, 1–47.
Russell, M. A. H., Peto, J., and Patel, A. (1974) The classification of smoking by factorial structure of motives.Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 137, 313–346.
Schalling, D. (1978) Psychopathy-related personality variables and the psychophysiology of socialization. In R. D. Hare and D. Schalling (Eds.),Psychopathic Behaviour: Approaches to Research. Chichester: Wiley, 85–107.
Shiffman, S. M., and Jarvik, M. E. (1976) Smoking withdrawal symptoms in two weeks of abstinence.Psychopharmacology, 50, 35–39.
Weil, A. T., Zinberg, N. E., and Nelson, J. M. (1968) Clinical and psychological effects of marihuana in man.Science, 162, 1234–1242.
Zenhausern, R. (1978) Imagery, cerebral dominance, and style of thinking: a unified field model.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 12, 381–384.
Zinberg, N. E. (1984)Drug, Set, and Setting. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Additional information
Supported in part by University grant 1322.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Lyvers, M., Boyd, G. & Maltzman, I. Smoking, personality, and imagery preference in Relation to spontaneous bilateral electrodermal activity. Pav. J. Biol. Sci. 22, 7–15 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02699435
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02699435