Abstract
It is proposed here that there is a sensitive period in the first two to three years of life during which humans acquire a basic knowledge of what foods are safe to eat. In support of this, it is shown that willingness to eat a wide variety of foods is greatest between the ages of one and two years, and then declines to low levels by age four. These data also show that children who are introduced to solids unusually late have a narrower diet breadth throughout childhood, perhaps because the duration of the sensitive period has been shortened. By reducing the costs associated with learning, a sensitive period for food learning should be adaptive for any omnivore (including early humans) that remains in the same environment throughout its life.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
References
Birch, L. L. 1979 Dimensions of Preschool Children’s Food Preferences.Journal of Nutrition Education 11:77–80.
1980 Effects of Peer Models’ Food Choices and Eating Behaviors on Preschoolers’ Food Preferences.Child Development 51:489–496.
1987 The Acquisition of Food Acceptance Patterns in Children. InEating Habits: Food, Physiology and Learned Behavior, R. Boakes, D. Popplewell, and M. Burton, eds. Pp. 107–130. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Birch, L. L., and D. W. Marlin 1982 I Don’t Like It; I Never Tried It: Effects of Exposure on 2-Year-Old Children’s Food Preferences.Appetite 3:353–360.
Capretta, P. J., and L. Rawls 1974 Establishment of a Flavor Preference in Rats: Importance of Nursing and Weaning Experience.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 86:670–673.
Crawley, M. J. 1983Herbivory: The Dynamics of Animal-Plant Interactions. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Duncker, K. 1938 Experimental Modification of Children’s Food Preferences through Social Suggestion.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 3:489–508.
Fallon, A. E., P. Rozin, and P. Pliner 1984 The Child’s Conception of Food: The Development of Food Rejections with Special Reference to Disgust and Contamination Sensitivity.Child Development 55:566–575.
Farb, P., and G. Armelagos 1980Consuming Passions: The Anthropology of Eating. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Fomon, S. J. 1974Infant Nutrition. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Galef, B. J. 1991 Innovation in the Study of Social Learning in Animals: Developmental and Biological Perspectives. InDevelopmental Psychobiology: New Methods and Changing Concepts, H. Shair, G. Barr, and M. Hofer, eds. Pp. 114–125. New York: Oxford University Press.
Galef, B. J., and M. Clark 1971 Social Factors in the Poison Avoidance and Feeding Behavior of Wild and Domesticated Rat Pups.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 5:341–357.
Galef, B. J., and D. F. Sherry 1973 Mother’s Milk: A Medium for the Transmission of Cues Reflecting the Flavor of Mother’s Diet.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 83:374–378.
Garcia, J., and R. A. Koelling 1966 Relation of Cue to Consequence in Avoidance Learning.Psychonomic Science 4:123–124.
Harper, L. V., and K. M. Sanders 1975 The Effect of Adults’ Eating on Young Children’s Acceptance of Unfamiliar Foods.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 20:206–214.
Kram, K. M., and G. M. Owen 1972 Nutritional Studies on United States Preschool Children: Dietary Intakes and Practices of Food Procurement, Preparation and Consumption. InPractices of Low-Income Families in Feeding Infants and Small Children, with Particular Attention to Cultural Subgroups, S. Fomon and T. Anderson, eds. HEW Publication No. 72-5605. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Lebenthal, E. 1982 Solid Food (Beikost) Consumption and Digestibility in Infancy. InPediatric Nutrition, F. Lifshitz, ed. Pp. 63–69. New York: Marcel Dekker.
Logue, A. W., M. C. Logue, R. Uzzo, M. McCarty, and M. Smith 1988 Food Preferences in Families.Appetite 10:169–180.
McKenzie, J. C. 1967 Social and Economic Implications of Minority Food Habits.Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 26:197–205.
Pelchat, M. L., and P. Pliner 1986 Antecedents and Correlates of Feeding Problems in Young Children.Journal of Nutrition Education 18:23–29.
Peryam, D. R. 1963 The Acceptance of Novel Foods.Food Technology 17:33–39.
Pliner, P. 1982 The Effects of Mere Exposure on Liking for Edible Substances.Appetite 3:283–290.
Pulliam, H. R., and C. Dunford 1979Programmed to Learn: An Essay on the Evolution of Culture. New York: Columbia University Press.
Rabinowitch, V. 1969 Role of Experience in the Development and Retention of Seed Preferences in Zebra Finches.Behaviour 33:222–236.
Raphael, D. 1979Breastfeeding and Food Policy in a Hungry World. New York: Academic Press.
Raphael, D., and F. Davis 1985Only Mothers Know: Patterns of Infant Feeding in Traditional Cultures. Westport, Connecticut: Greenwood Press.
Rosenthal, G. A., and D. H. Janzen, eds. 1979Herbivores: Their Interaction with Secondary Plant Metabolites. New York: Academic Press.
Rozin, P. 1976 Psychobiological and Cultural Determinants of Food Choice. InAppetite and Food Intake, T. Silverstone, ed. Pp. 285–312. Berlin: Dahlem Konferenzen.
Rozin, P., A. Fallon, and M. Augustoni-Ziskind 1986 The Child’s Conception of Food: The Development of Categories of Acceptable and Rejected Substances.Journal of Nutrition Education 18:75–81.
Rozin, P., L. Hammer, H. Oster, T. Horowitz, and V. Marmora 1986 The Child’s Conception of Food: Differentiation of Categories of Rejected Substances in the 16 Months to 5 Year Age Range.Appetite 7:141–151.
Slater, P. J. B. 1983 The Development of Individual Behavior. InGenes, Development and Learning, T. R. Halliday and P. Slater, eds. Pp. 82–113. Oxford: Blackwell.
Stephens, D. W. 1990 Risk and Incomplete Information in Behavioral Ecology. InRisk and Uncertainty in Tribal and Peasant Economies, E. Cashdan, ed. Pp. 19–46. Boulder: Westview Press.
1991 Change, Regularity and Value in the Evolution of Animal Learning.Journal of Behavioural Ecology 2:77–89.
Symons, D. 1979The Evolution of Human Sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.
Wilson, E. O. 1975Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
Wyrwicka, W. 1981The Development of Food Preferences: Parental Influences and the Primacy Effect. Springfield, Illinois: Charles Thomas.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Additional information
The data were collected with the assistance of faculty and parents from Rowland Hall-St. Mark’s School, the Jewish Community Center of Salt Lake City, and the University of Utah Child and Family Development Center.
Elizabeth Cashdan is Associate Professor of Anthropology at the University of Utah. In addition to the evolution of food learning, she is interested in hunter-gatherer ecology, human reproductive strategies, and the endocrine correlates of dominance in women. She has worked with foragers and farmers in Botswana and with children and adults in Salt Lake City.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Cashdan, E. A sensitive period for learning about food. Human Nature 5, 279–291 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02692155
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02692155
Key words
- Sensitive periods
- Critical periods
- Sensitive period learning
- Food learning
- Food preferences
- Weaning