Skip to main content
Log in

Richard L. Solomon and learned helplessness

  • Historical Section
  • Published:
Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article reviews the course of development of research on a currently popular explanatory approach to dysfunctional behavior, the learned helplessness analysis. The early history is prominent in this review as it reflects the inspirations of Richard L. Solomon, a scholar who fostered the resurgence of psychologists’ interests in Pavlovian conditioning in the 1950s and 1960s. Current research is characterized as having four separate themes: elaboration of “symptoms,” elucidating the role of fear, explicit modeling, and extensions involving attributional constructs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P. & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L.B., & Bersh, P.J. (1979). Partial control and learned helplessness: Control over shock intensity prevents interference with subsequent escape.Animal Learning & Behavior, 7, 157–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L.B., & Ehrman, R.N. (1981). Learning about response reinforcer contingencies affects subsequent acquisition of stimulus reinforcer contingencies.Learning & Motivation, 12, 109–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L.B., Peterson, C., Abramson, L.Y., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1984) Attributional style and the generality of learned helplessness.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 681–687.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L.B., & Tabachnik, N. (1984). Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information.Psychological Review, 91, 112–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anisman, H., & Sklar, L. (1979). Catecholamine depletion in mice upon reexposure to stress: Mediation of the escape deficits produced by inescapable shock.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 93, 610–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bersh, P.J., Whitehouse, W.G., Laurence, M.T., Blaustein, J.E., & Alloy, L.B. (1990). Signalling the duration of uncontrolled shock impairs subsequent shock escape.Psychological Record, 40, 113–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, A.H., Solomon, R.L., Whiting, J.W.M. (1954). Resistance to temptation as a function of antecedent dependency relationships in puppies. Paper presented at Eastern Psychological Association meeting, New York, April.American Psychologist, 9, 579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brush, F.R., Del Plaine, S.N., Pelligrino, L.J., Rykaszewski, I.M., Dess, N.K., & Collins, P.Y. (1988). CER suppression, passive avoidance learning, and stress-induced suppression of drinking in the Syracuse High- and Low-Avoidance strains of rats (Rattus norvegicus).Journal of Comparative Psychology, 102, 337–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M., LoLordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Solomon, R. L., and Turner, L. H. (1966). Cardiac responses to shock in curarized dogs: Effects of shock intensity and duration, warning signal, and prior experience with shock.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 62, 1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dess, N.K., & Chapman, C.D. (1990). Individual differences in taste, body weight, and depression in the “helplessness” rat model and in humans.Brain Research Bulletin, 24, 669–676.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dess, N.K., Chapman, C.D., & Minor, T.R. (1988). Inescapable shock increases finickiness about drinking quinine adulterated water in rats.Learning & Motivation, 19, 408–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dess, N. K., and Overmier, J. B. (1989). Generalized learned irrelevance: Proactive effects on Pavlovian conditioning of dogs.Learning and Motivation, 20, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dorworth, T., and Overmier, J. B. (1977). On learned helplessness: The therapeutic effects of electroconvulsive shocks.Physiological Psychology, 5, 355–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haracz, J.L., Minor, T.R., Wilkins, J.N., & Zimmerman, E.G. (1988). Learned helplessness: An experimental model of the DST in rats.Biological Psychiatry, 23, 388–396. [But see Murison & Overmier. (1989). Animal models: Promises and pitfalls.Biol. Psychiatry, 26, 431–436.]

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hiroto, D. S. (1974). Locus of control and learned helplessness.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, R.L., Alexander, J.H. & Maier, S.F. (1980). Learned helplessness, inactivity, and associative deficits: The effects of inescapable shock on response choice escape learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 6, 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, D.C., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1976). Reversal of performance deficits and perceptual deficits in learned helplessness and depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 11–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laudenslager, M.L., Ryan, S.M., Drugan, R.C., Hyson, R.L., & Maier, S.F. (1983). Coping and immunosuppression: Inescapable shock but not escapable shock suppresses lymphocyte proliferation.Science, 221, 568–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leshner, A.I., Remler, H., Biegon, & Samuel, D. (1979). The antidepressant desmethylimipramine (DMI) counteracts learned helplessness in rats.Psychopharmacology, 66, 207–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maier, S.F., Drugan, R. Grau, J.W., Hyson, R., MacLennan, A.J., Moye, T., Madden J. IV, & Barchas, J.D. (1983). Learned helplessness, pain inhibition, and endogenous opiates.Advances in Analysis of Behavior, 3, 275–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, S.F., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1976). Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105:3–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maier, S.F., Seligman, R.L., Solomon, R.L. (1969). Pavlovian fear conditioning and learned helplessness: Effects on escape and avoidance behavior of (a) the CS-US contingency and (b) the independence of US and voluntary responding. In B.A. Campbell & R.N. Church (Eds.),Punishment and Aversive Behavior New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 299–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, S. F., Sherman, J. E., Lewis, J. W., Terman, G. W., & Liebeskind, J. C. (1983). The opioid/nonopioid nature of stress-induced analgesia and learned helplessness.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 80–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., Cook, M., & Miller, S. (1984). Fear conditioned with escapable and inescapable shock: Effects of a feedback stimulus.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 307–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minor, T. R. (1990). Conditioned fear and neophobia following inescapable shock.Animal Learning & Behavior, 18, 212–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minor, T.R., Dess, N.K., Ben-David, E., & Chang, W-C. (1994). Individual Differences in vulnerability to inescapable shock in rats.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 20, 402–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minor, T. R., Trauner, M. A., Lee, C., & Dess, N. K. (1990). Modeling signal features of escape response: Effects of cessation conditioning in “Learned Helplessness” paradigm.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 123–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minor, T.R., Chang, W., & Winslow, J.L. (1994). Stress and adenosine: I. Effect of methylxanthine and amphetamine stimulants on learned helplessness in rats.Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 254–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O.H. (1947) On the dual nature of learning—a reinterpretation of “conditioning” and “problem solving.”Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J.B. (1968) Interference with avoidance behavior.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78: 340–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J. B., and Leaf, R. C. (1965). Effects of discriminative Pavlovian fear conditioning upon previously or subsequently acquired avoidance responding.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 60, 213–217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J.B., Murison, R., Skoglund, E. J., and Ursin, H. (1985). Safety signals can mimic responses in reducing the ulcerogenic effects of shock.Physiological Psychology, 13, 243–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J.B., Murison, R., Taklo, T., & Esplid, R. (1994). Effects of traumatic stress on defensive burying: An alternative test of the learned helplessness model of depression and enhanced retrieval of unpleasant memories.Biological Psychiatry, 36, 703–704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J. B., and Patterson, J. (1988). Animal models of psychopathology. In P. Soubrie, P. Simon, and D. Widlocher (Eds).Animal Models of Psychiatric Disorders, Vol. I, Selected Models of Anxiety, Depression and Psychosis, Basel, Sw.: Karger, pp. 1–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J. B., and Seligman, M. E. P. (1967). Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 28–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J. B., and Wielkiewicz, R. M. (1983) On unpredictability as a causal factor in “Learned Helplessness.”Learning and Motivation, 14, 324–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. (1988) Explanatory style as a risk factor for illness.Cognitive Therapy & Research, 12, 117–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence.Psychological Review, 91, 347–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, F., & Sherman, A.D. (1983). Learned helplessness induction decreases in vivo cortical serotonin release.Pharmacology, Biochemistry, & Behavior, 18, 649–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pucilowski, O., Overstreet, D.H., Rezvani, A.H., & Janowski, D.S. (1993). Chronic mild stress-induced anhedonia: Greater effect in genetic rat model of depression.Physiology & Behavior, 54, 1215–1220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosellini, R.A., Warren, D.A., & DeCola, J.P. (1987) Predictability and controllability: Differential effects upon contextual fear.Learning & Motivation, 18, 392–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, F., Gur, G.E., Alavi, A., Seligman, M.E.P., Mozley, L., Smith, R.J., Mozley, P., & Gur, R. (1996). Cerebral bloodflow changes in limbic regions induced by unsolvable anagram tasks.American Journal of Psychiatry. (In press.)

  • Seligman, M.E.P. (1975).Helplessness. On Depression, Dying, and Death. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P. (1991).Learned Optimism. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P., & Maier, S.F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sklar, L.S., & Animan, H. (1981). Stress and cancer.Psychological Bulletin, 89, 369–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R.L. (1967). Aversive control in relation to the development of behavior disorders. In J. Zubin & H.F. Hunt (Eds.),Comparative Psychopathology: Animals and Human. New York: Grune & Straton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R.L., & Corbit, J.D. (1974). An opponent process theory of motivation: I. The temporal dynamics of affect.Psychological Review, 81, 119–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R.L., & Turner, L.H. (1960). Discriminative classical conditioning under curare can later control discriminative avoidance responses in the normal state.Science, 132, 1499–1500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R.L., & Wynne, L.C. (1954). Traumatic avoidance learning: The principles of anxiety conservation and partial irreversibility.Psychological Review, 61, 353–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, J.M. (1971). Effects of coping behavior in different signal conditions on stress pathology.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 77, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, J.M., Goodman, P.A., Losito, B.G., Corrigan, S., Charry, J.M., & Bailey, W.H. (1981). Behavioral depression produced by an uncontrollable stressor: Relationship to norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin levels in rats.Brain Research Reviews, 3, 167–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J.L. (1982). Influence of shock controllability by dominant rats on subsequent attack and defensive behaviors towards colony intruders.Animal Learning & Behavior, 10, 305–313.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Overmier, J.B. Richard L. Solomon and learned helplessness. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science 31, 331–337 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02691436

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02691436

Keywords

Navigation