Skip to main content
Log in

A model of pavlovian conditioning: Variations in representations of the unconditional stimulus

  • Papers
  • Published:
Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We present a model of Pavlovian excitatory conditioning in which associative strength and malleable central representations of unconditional stimuli determine the strength of conditional responding. Presentation of a conditioned stimulus acts through an experientially determined associative bond to activate a representation of the unconditional stimulus. The activation of the representation produces a conditioned response. A striking feature of the model is its ability to describe changes in conditioned response magnitude in terms of alterations of representations of the unconditional stimulus. Another is its acknowledgement of the capacity of associative bonds to survive behavioral extinction. The model describes much of the data reported from excitatory conditioning experiments and predicts counterintuitive phenomena.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Annau, Z, & Kamin, L. J. (1961). The conditioned emotional response as a function of the intensity of the U.S.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54, 428–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bain, A.G. (1855).The senses and the intellect. London: Parker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, A., & Mackintosh, N.J. (1979). Preexposure to the CS alone, US alone, or CS and US uncorrelated: Latent inhibition, blocking by context, or learned irrelevance?Learning and Motivation, 10, 278–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bindra, D. (1978). How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement.The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 41–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackburn, J.R., & Jacobs, W.J. (unpublished data). Incubation of conditioned suppression is a variant of inflation.

  • Bolles, R.C. (1975). Learning, motivation, and cognition. In W.K. Estes (Ed.),Handbook of learning and cognitive processes, Vol. 1: Introduction to concepts and issues, (pp. 249–280). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolles, R.C. (1967).Theory of motivation. New York: Harper Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouton, M.E. (1986). Slow reacquisition following the extinction of conditioned suppression.Learning and Motivation, 17, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouton, M.E., & Boles, R.C. (1979). Role of conditioned contextual stimuli in reinstatement of extinguished fear.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 368–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bouton, M.E., & King, D.A. (1986). Effect of context on performance to conditioned stimuli with mixed histories of reinforcement and nonreinforcement.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 4–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. (1824).Lectures on the philosophy of the human mind. (Ed.), Vol. 2. Edinbourough: Tait.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bush, R.R., & Mosteller, F. (1955).Stochastic models for learning. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, J.H. (1987). Cellular analysis of associative learning.Physiological Reviews, 67, 329–439.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colgan, D.M. (1970). Effect of instructions on the skin resistance response.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 108–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damianopolous, E.N. (1987). Conditioning, attention, and the CS-UCS interval: A theoretical statement of relations.Pavlovian Journal of Biological Psychology, 22, 16–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M.E., & Grings, W.W. (1968). Comparison of classical conditioning and relational learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 227–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A., & Mackintosh, N.J. (1979). Reinforcer specificity in the enhancement of conditioning by posttrial surprise.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 162–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domjan, M. & Galef, B.G. (1983). Biological constraints on instrumental and classical conditioning: Retrospect and prospect.Animal Learning & Behavior, 11, 151–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlap, K. (1932).Habits: Their making and unmaking. New York: Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eich, J.M. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model.Psychological Review, 89, 627–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eich, J.M. (1985). Levels of processing, encoding specificity, elaboration, and CHARM.Psychological Review, 92, 1–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E.B., & Kozak, M.J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information.Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, P.W., & Butler, C.S. (1977). Extinction after aversive conditioning: An associative or nonassociative process?Learning and Motivation, 8, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, P.W., & Sears, R.J. (1978). Model of conditioning incorporating the Rescorla-Wagner associative axiom, a dynamic attention process, and a catastrophe rule.Psychological Review, 85, 321–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gluck, M.A., & Thompson, R.F. (1987). Modeling the neural substrates of associative learning and memory: A computational approach.Psychological Review, 94, 176–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, E.R. (1935).The psychology of learning (2nd Ed.), 1952. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grings, W.W. (1973). Cognitive factors in electrodermal conditioning.Psychological Bulletin, 79, 200–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, A. (1982). Processing of expected and unexpected events during conditioning and attention: A psychophysiological theory.Psychological Review, 89, 529–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, D. (1749).Observations on man, his frame, his duty, and his expectations. London: Leaker and Frederick.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P.C. (1977). Conditioned stimulus as a determinant of the form of the Pavlovian conditioned response.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, 77–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P.C. (1984). Origins of behavior in Pavlovian conditioning.The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 18, 129–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C.L. (1943).Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutcheson, F. (1971).An inquiry into the original of our ideas of beauty and virtue. New York: Garland. (Original work published 1716).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1965).A treatise of human nature. L. A. Selby-Bigge (Ed.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1739).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.J., & Blackburn, J.R. (1987). Incubation of conditional suppression is an associatively-based phenomenon.Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 22, 118–121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.J., & Blackburn, J.R. (1988). Factors contributing to the magnitude of conditional fear following a 24-h retention interval: The incubation effect.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26, 145–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.J., Buttrick, M., & Kennedy, D. (1988). A rapid and sensitive method for measuring the conditioned emotional response II: On-the-baseline excitatory conditioning and extinction.Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 23, 29–34.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.J. & LoLordo, V.M. (1980). Selective association in aversive conditioning: Implications for avoidance learning in the rat.Learning and Motivation, 11, 427–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.J., Nadel, L. & Hayden, V. (1993). Anxiety disorders. In D. J. Stein & J. Young (Eds.),Cognitive science and clinical disorders, (pp. 211–233). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.J., & Nadel, L. (1985). Stress induced recovery of fears and phobias.Psychological Review, 92, 512–531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1981).Principles of psychology. F.M. Burkhardt (Ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1890).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamin, L.J. (1965). Temporal and intensity characteristics of the conditioned stimulus. In W. F. Prokasy (Ed.).Classical conditioning: A symposium, (pp. 118–147). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamin, L.J. (1969). Selective association and conditioning. In N.J. Mackintosh & W.K. Honig (Eds.),Fundamental issues in associative learning, (pp. 42–64). Halifax: Dalhousie University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klopf, A.H. (1988). A neuronal model of classical conditioning.Psychobiology, 16, 85–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. (1948).Conditioned reflexes and neuron organization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. (1967).Integrative activity of the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J., & Szwejkowska, G. (1952). Chronic extinction and restoration of conditioned reflexes: IV. The dependence of the course of extinction and restoration of conditioned reflexes on the “history” of the conditioned stimulus. (The principle of the primacy of first training.)Acta Biologiae Experimentalis, 16, 95–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J., & Szwejkowska, G. (1956). Reciprocal transformation of heterogeneous conditioned reflexes.Acta Biologiae Experimentalis, 17, 141–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P.J. (1977). Imagery in therapy: An information processing analysis of fear.Behavior Therapy, 8, 862–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P.J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery.Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P.J., Kozak, M.J., Miller, G.A., Levin, D.N., & McLean, A., Jr. (1980). Emotional imagery: Conceptual structure and pattern of somatovisceral response.Psychophysiology, 17, 179–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K.S. (1929).Brain mechanisms and intelligence: A quantitative study of injuries to the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. (1959).An essay concerning human understanding. A.C. Fraser (Ed.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1700).

    Google Scholar 

  • LoLordo, V.M. & Jacobs, W.J. (1983). Constraints on aversive conditioning in the rat: Some theoretical accounts. In P. Harzem & M. Zeiler (Eds.),Advances in the Analysis of Behavior (pp. 325–350). Wiley: London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubow, R.E. (1973). Latent inhibition.Psychological Bulletin, 79, 398–407.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lubow, R.E., Weiner, I. & Schnur, P. (1981). Conditioned attention theory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation, vol 15 (pp. 1–49). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (1973). Stimulus selection: Learning to ignore stimuli that predict no change in reinforcement. In R.A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.),Constraints on learning, (pp. 75–96). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (1974).The psychology of animal learning. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (1975a). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement.Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (1975b). Blocking of conditioned suppression: Role of the first compound trial.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 335–345.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (1983).Conditioning and associative learning. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J., Bygrave, D.J., & Picton, B.M.B. (1977). Locus of the effect of a surprising reinforcer in the attenuation of blocking.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 327–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, L., & Rumelhart, D.E. (1985). Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 189–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matzel, L.D., Schachtman, T.R., & Miller R.R. (1988). Learned irrelevance exceeds the sum of CS-preexposure and UCS-preexposure deficits.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 311–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalf-Eich, J. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model.Psychological Review, 89, 627–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalf-Eich, J. (1985). Levels of processing, encoding specificity, elaboration, and CHARM.Psychological Review, 92, 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalf, J. (1991). Recognition failure and the composite memory trace in CHARM.Psychological Review, 98, 529–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J.S. (1973).A system of logic, ratiocinative and inductive. (J.M. Robson, Ed.). Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (Original work published 1843).

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, R.R., & Matzel, L.D. (1988). The comparator hypothesis: A response rule for the expression of association. In G.H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation, 22. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, L., & Willner, J. (1980). Context and conditioning: A place for space.Physiological Psychology, 8, 218–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I.P. (1960).Conditioned reflexes. (G.V. Anrep, Ed. & Trans.). New York: Dover. (Original work published, 1927).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, J.M. (1987). A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning.Psychological Review, 94, 61–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, J.M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli.Psychological Review, 87, 532–552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prokasy, W.F., & Allan, C.K. (1969). Instruction sets in human differential eyelid conditioning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 271–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Randich, A., & LoLordo, V.M. (1979). Associative and non-associative theories of the UCS preexposure phenomenon: Implications for Pavlovian conditioning.Psychological Bulletin, 86, 523–548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1968). Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 1–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1973). Effect of US habituation following conditioning.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 137–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A. (1974). Effect of inflation of the unconditioned stimulus value following conditioning.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 101–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A., & Heth, C.D. (1975). Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 88–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A., & LoLordo, V.M. (1965). Inhibition of avoidance behavior.Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 59, 406–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A, & Wagner, A.R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A.H. Black & W.F. Prokasy (Eds.),Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, R.T., & LoLordo, V.M. (1987). Evaluation of the relation between Pavlovian occasion-setting and instrumental discriminative stimuli: A blocking analysis.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 13, 3–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning.Psychological Review, 77, 406–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, J.E. (1978). US inflation with trace and simultaneous fear conditioning.Animal Learning and Behavior, 6, 463–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S.J. (1983). Function and mechanism in learning. In M. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.),Advances in the analysis of behavior: Biological factors in learning (pp. 1–39). London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E.N. (1963).Perception and the conditioned reflex. (S.W. Waydenfeld, trans.). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, N.S., & Mackintosh, N.J. (1971).Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, R.S., & Bartow, A.G. (1981). Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: Expectation and prediction.Psychological Review, 88, 135–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Testa, T.J. (1974). Causal relationships and the acquisition of avoidance responses.Psychological Review, 81, 491–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E.L. (1911).Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vigorito, M., & Ayres, J.J.B. (1987). Effect of naloxone on conditioned suppression in rats.Behavioral Neuroscience, 101, 576–586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, A.R. (1978). Expectancies and the priming of STM. In S.H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & W.K. Honig (Eds.),Cognitive processes in animal behavior (pp. 177–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, A.R. (1981). SOP: A model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. In N.E. Spear & R.R. Miller (Eds.),Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms (pp. 5–47). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, A.R., & Rescorla, R.A. (1972). Inhibition in Pavlovian conditioning: Application of a theory. In R. A. Boakes & M.S. Halliday (Eds.),Inhibition and learning (pp. 301–336). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it.Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.B. (1924).Behaviorism. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W.M. (1902).Principles of physiological psychology. (E.B. Titchener, Trans.) New York: Macmillan. (Original work published 1874)

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to W. Jake Jacobs or James R. Blackburn.

Additional information

This work was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council grant #U0262 awarded to the first author.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jacobs, W.J., Blackburn, J.R. A model of pavlovian conditioning: Variations in representations of the unconditional stimulus. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science 30, 12–33 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02691387

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02691387

Keywords

Navigation