Skip to main content
Log in

Pavlovian conditioning in human skilled motor behavior

  • Published:
Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The role of Pavlovian contingencies in human skilled motor behavior was investigated in three experiments by means of a new conditioning preparation. In Experiment 1 the present method was shown to be appropriate for the study of associative learning. Subjects who experienced a standard delay configuration performed significantly more conditioned responses than subjects who received either backward conditioning or random pairings. Stimulus generalization was shown to be slight in two additional groups. Subsequent experiments examined conditioning with multiple conditioned stimuli (CSs). In particular, in Experiment 2 some reciprocal overshadowing was demonstrated when two conditional stimuli (tone and vibration) were compounded. Experiment 3 investigated blocking. Blocking was less than expected, however. Subjects’ perceptions of the stimuli and reaction time data suggest that a certain proportion had shifted their attention to the added element of the CS compound. Results are discussed in relation to other studies on Pavlovian learning in humans and animals, which are concerned with “stimulus selection.”

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baer, P.E., & Fuhrer, M.J. (1969). Cognitive factors in differential conditioning of the GSR: Use of a reaction time task as the UCS with normals and schizophrenics.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74, 544–552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard, R., & Honig, W.K. (1976). Surprise value of food determines its effectiveness as a reinforcer.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, (2), 67–74.

  • Bond, N.W. (1983). Reciprocal overshadowing in flavour-aversion learning.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(B), 265–274.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davey, G.C.L. (1987). An integration of human and animal models of Pavlovian conditioning: Associations, cognitions, and attributions. In G. Davey (Ed.),Cognitive processes and pavlovian conditioning in humans (pp. 83–114). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M.E., & Schell, A.M. (1987). Human autonomic and skeletal classical conditioning: The role of conscious factors. In G. Davey (Ed.),Cognitive processes and pavlovian conditioning in humans (pp. 27–55). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dmitriev, A.S. (1962). On a motor technique with verbal reinforcement.Soviet Psychology and Psychiatry, 1, 8–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J.M. (1971). Added cue control as a function of reinforcement predictability.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 91, 318–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleiss, J.L. (1981).Statistical methods for rates and proportions. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furedy, J.J. (1991). Some recalcitrant views on the role of non-cognitive S-R factors in human autonomic conditioning.Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 26(1), 21–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gormezano, I., & Kehoe, E.J. (1975). Classical conditioning: Some methodological-conceptual issues. In W.K. Estes (Ed.),Handbook of learning and cognitive processes, Vol. 2 (pp. 143–179). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C.L. (1943).Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivanov-Smolenski, A.G. (1927). On the methods of examining the conditioned food reflexes in children and in mental disorders.Brain, 50, 138–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, J.H., & Wagner, A.R. (1980). One-trial overshadowing: Evidence of distributive processing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 6(2), 183–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamin, L.J. (1968). “Attention-like” processes in classical conditioning. In M.R. Jones (Ed.),Miami Symposion on the prediction of behavior: Aversive stimulation (pp. 9–33). Miami: University of Miami Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, M.A., & Bolles, R.C. (1981). A nonassociative aspect of overshadowing.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18(b), 318–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipp, O.V., & Vaitl, D. (1988). Reaction time task as unconditioned stimulus: On conditioning skin conductance responses and heart rate using a nonaversive unconditional stimulus.Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 23, 165–172

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovibond, P.F., Siddle, D.A.T., & Bond, N. (1988). Insensitivity to stimulus validity in human Pavlovian conditioning.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40B(4), 377–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement.Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, I., & Levey, A.B. (1987). Learning what will happen next: Conditioning, evaluation, and cognitive processes. In G. Davey (Ed.),Cognitive processes and pavlovian conditioning in humans (pp. 57–82). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, I., & Levey, A.B. (1991). Blocking observed in human subjects.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B(3), 233–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mis, F.W., & Moore, J.W. (1973). Effects of preacquisition UCS exposure on classical conditioning of the rabbits’ nictitating membrane response.Learning and Motivation, 4, 108–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papini, M.R., & Bitterman, M.E. (1990). The role of contingency in classical conditioning.Psychological Review, 97(3), 396–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I.P. (1927).Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A., & Wagner, A.R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A.H. Black & B.F. Prokasy (Eds.),Classical conditioning, Vol. 2 (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K.W. (1966). Cognitive and drive factors in the extinction of the conditioned eyelid response in human subjects.Psychological Review, 73, 445–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J.E.R. (1983).Adaptive behavior and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomie, A. (1980). Effects of unpredictable food upon the subsequent acquisition of autoshaping: Analysis of the context blocking hypothesis. In C.M. Locurto, H.S. Terrace, & J. Gibbon (Eds.),Autoshaping and conditioning theory (pp. 181–215). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso, T., & Bower, G.H. (1968).Attention and Learning. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rübeling, H. Pavlovian conditioning in human skilled motor behavior. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science 28, 29–45 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02691198

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02691198

Key Words

Navigation