Advertisement

Educational Technology Research and Development

, Volume 53, Issue 3, pp 73–81 | Cite as

Instructional design for advanced learners: Establishing connections between the theoretical frameworks of cognitive load and deliberate practice

  • Tamara van Gog
  • K. Anders Ericsson
  • Remy M. J. P. Rikers
  • Fred Paas
Special Issue

Abstract

Cognitive load theory (CLT) has been successful in identifying instructional formats that are more effective and efficient than conventional problem solving in the initial, novice phase of skill acquisition. However, recent findings regarding the “expertise reversal effect” have begun to stimulate cognitive load theorists to broaden their horizon to the question of how instructional design should be altered as a learner's knowledge increases. To answer this question, it is important to understand how expertise is acquired and what fosters its development. Expert performance research, and, in particular, the theoretical framework of deliberate practice have given us a better understanding of the principles and activities that are essential in order to excel in a domain. This article explores how these activities and principles can be used to design instructional formats based on CLT for higher levels of skills mastery. The value of these formats for e-learning environments in which learning tasks can be adaptively selected on the basis of online assessments of the learner's level of expertise is discussed.

Keywords

Cognitive Load Instructional Design Cognitive Load Theory Deliberate Practice Expert Performance 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Camp, G., Paas, F., Rikers, R.M.J.P., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2001). Dynamic problem selection in air traffic control training: A comparison between performance, mental effort and mental efficiency.Computers in Human Behavior, 17, 575–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction.Cognition and Instruction, 8, 293–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Charness, N., Reingold, E.M., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D.M. (2001). The perceptual aspect of skilled performance in chess: Evidence from eye movements.Memory & Cognition, 29, 1146–1152.Google Scholar
  4. Chi, M.T.H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. (Eds.), (1988).The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Cooper, G., Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learning by imagining.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 68–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Deakin, J.M., & Cobley, S. (2003). A search for deliberate practice: An examination of the practice environment in figure skating and volleyball. In J. Starkes & K.A. Ericsson (Eds.),Expert performance in sport: Recent advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 115–132). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
  7. Ericsson, K.A. (2002). Attaining excellence through deliberate practice: Insights from the study of expert performance. In M. Ferrari (Ed.),The pursuit of excellence through education (pp. 21–55). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Ericsson, K.A. (2003). How the expert-performance approach differs from traditional approaches to expertise in sports: In search of a shared theoretical framework for studying expert performance. In J. Starkes & K.A. Ericsson (Eds.),Expert performance in sport: Recent advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 371–401). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
  9. Ericsson, K.A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory.Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance.Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Ericsson, K.A., & Lehmann, A.C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence for maximal adaptation to task constraints.Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ericsson, K.A., & Simon, H.A. (1993).Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (Rev. ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  13. Fitts, P.M., & Posner, M.I. (1967).Human Performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
  14. Gerjets, P., & Scheiter, K. (2003). Goal configurations and processing strategies as moderators between instructional design and cognitive load: Evidence from hypertext-based instruction.Educational Psychologist, 38, 33–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Glaser, R. (1996). Changing the agency for learning: Acquiring expert performance. In K.A. Ericsson (Ed.),The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 303–311). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  16. Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect.Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional design.Human Factors, 40, 1–17.Google Scholar
  18. Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2005). Rapid dynamic assessment of expertise to improve the efficiency of adaptive e-learning. [This special issue].Educational Technology, Research and Development 53(3), 83–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive load approach.Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments.Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Paas, F., Tuovinen, J., van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Darabi, A. (2005). A motivational perspective on the relation between mental effort and performance: Optimizing learners' involvement in instructional conditions. [This special issue].Educational Technology, Research and Development, 53(3), 25–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Renkl, A. (2002). Worked-out examples: instructional explanations support learning by self-explanations.Learning and Instruction, 12, 529–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Rikers, R.M.J.P., Schmidt, H.G., & Boshuizen, H.P.A. (2002). On the constraints of encapsulated knowledge: Clinical case representations by medical experts and subexperts.Cognition and Instruction, 20, 27–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rikers, R.M.J.P., van Gerven, P.W.M., & Schmidt, H.G. (2004). Cognitive load theory as a tool for expertise development.Instructional Science, 32, 173–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Salden, R.J.C.M., Paas, F., Broers, N.J., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Mental effort and performance as determinants for the dynamic selection of learning tasks in air traffic control training.Instructional Science, 32, 153–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Shute, V., & Towle, B. (2003). Adaptive e-learningEducational Psychologist, 38, 105–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning.Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture.Instructional Science, 32, 9–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design.Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding.Instructional Science, 32, 83–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Zhu, X., & Simon, H.A. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing.Cognition and Instruction, 4, 137–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Zimmerman, B.J. (2002). Achieving academic excellence: A self-regulatory perspective. In M. Ferrari (Ed.),The pursuit of excellence through education (pp. 85–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Educational Communications and Technology 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Tamara van Gog
    • 3
  • K. Anders Ericsson
    • 1
  • Remy M. J. P. Rikers
    • 2
  • Fred Paas
    • 3
  1. 1.Psychology Department at Florida State UniversityUSA
  2. 2.Psychology Department at Erasmus University RotterdamThe Netherlands
  3. 3.Educational Technology Expertise CenterOpen University of the NetherlandsHeerlenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations