Skip to main content
Log in

The effects of visual and verbal coding mnemonics on learning Chinese characters in computer-based instruction

  • Research
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The effects of different approaches to learning Chinese characters were investigated. Ninety-two high-school students were randomly assigned to one of five treatment groups: translation, verbal mnemonics, visual mnemonics, dual coding mnemonics, or self-generated mnemonics. All groups received instruction and completed posttests in a computer-based environment. The results indicate that participants who generated their own mnemonics demonstrated higher posttest performance than those in visual coding, verbal coding, and translation groups; subjects in the dual coding group scored higher than those in the translation group. Those who generated their own mnemonics spent more time on task than any other group, and those in the verbal coding group took more time than those in the translation group. Survey and qualitative data suggest that learners' interpretations of the Chinese characters were rooted in their cultural backgrounds and personal experiences.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bednar, A.K., Cunningham, D., Duffy, T.M., & Perry, J.D. (1991). Theory into practice: How do we link? In Anglin, G.J. (Ed.),Instructional technology: Past, present, and future (pp. 88–101). Englewood, CO: Libraries Unlimited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellezza, A.S. (1981). Mnemonic devices: Classification, characteristics, and criteria.Review of Educational Research, 51(2), 247–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, C., & Blake, R. (1973, April).Cognitive scaffolding in the learning of foreign language vocabulary: An experimental study. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Educational Communications and Technology, Las Vegas, NV.

  • Carrier, C., Karbo, K., & Kindem, H. (1983). Use of selfgenerated and supplied visuals as mnemonics in gifted children's learning.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57(1), 235–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chuang, C.J. (1975). The function of imagery in learning of Chinese language.Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 1, 145–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, J., & Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory and education.Educational Psychology Review, 3(3), 149–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988).Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grabowski, B.L. (1996). Generative learning: Past, present, and future. In D.H. Jonassen (Ed.),Handbook of research in educational communications and technology (pp. 897–918). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. (1991). Evaluating constructivistic learning.Educational Technology, 30(9), 28–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ke, C. (1996). An empirical study on the relationship between Chinese character recognition and production.Modern Language Journal, 80(3), 340–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kibler, J.L., & Blick, K.A. (1972). Evaluation of experimenter-supplied and subject-originated first-letter mnemonic in a free-recall task.Psychological Reports, 30, 307–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, S. (1986). Theoretical issues concerning superiority of picture over words and sentences in memory.Perceptual and Motor Skill, 63, 783–792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, X. (1996). Hypercharacters: A pilot study in computerized learning of Chinese characters.CALICO Journal, 14(1), 77–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mantyla, T., & Nillson, L. (1983). Are my cues better then your cues?Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24, 303–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Anderson, R.B. (1991). Animations need narrations: An experimental test of dual-coding hypothesis.Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(4), 484–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Anderson, R.B. (1992). The instructional animation: helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(4), 444–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Moreno, R. (2002). Aids to computer-based multimedia learning.Learning and Instruction, 12(1), 107–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R.E., & Sims, V.K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia learning.Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 389–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGinnis, S. (1995, November).Does Johnny even want to read: Student attitudes and approaches in the learning of written Chinese. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Chinese Language Teachers Association, Anaheim, CA.

  • Paivio, A. (1971).Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1986).Mental representations: A dual-coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., & Lambert, W. (1981). Dual coding and bilingual memory.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(5), 532–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paradis, M. (1978). The stratification of bilingualism. In M. Paradis (Ed.),Aspects of bilingualism (pp. 165–176). Columbia, SC: Hornbean Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L.P. (1989). The effects of computer animated elaboration strategies and practice on factorial and application learning in an elementary science lesson.Journal of Educational Computing Research, 5(4), 431–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L.P. (1990). Animation and computer-based instruction.Educational Technology Research and Development, 38(1), 77–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L.P. (1991). Animation, incidental learning and continuing motivation.Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(3), 318–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L.P., & Kini, A.S. (1991). Theoretical foundations of instructional applications of computer-generated animated visuals.Journal of Computer-Based Instruction, 18(3), 83–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M., Goetz, E., & Avila, E. (1995). Concreteness effects in text recall: Dual coding or context available?Reading Research Quarterly, 30(2), 278–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M., Paivio, A., & Goetz, E. (1991). A critique of schema theory in reading and a dual coding alternative.Reading Research Quarterly, 26(4), 463–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffensen, M.S., Goetz, E.T., & Cheng, X. (1999) The images and emotions of bilingual Chinese readers: A dual coding analysis.Reading Psychology, 20(4), 301–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H.L. (1988). The effects of self-generated visual mnemonics on adult learning disabled readers' word recall.Learning Disabilities Research, 4(1), 26–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taura, H. (1998). Bilingual dual coding in Japanese returnee students.Language, Culture and Curriculum, 11(1), 47–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Towers, R.L., & Anglin, G.J. (1994, February).The instructional effectiveness of computer-aniimated and static visuals. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Educational Communications and Technology, Nashville, TN.

  • Turnage, T., & McGinnies, E. (1973). A cross-cultural comparison of the effects of presentation mode and meaningfulness on short-term recall.American Journal of Psychology, 86(2), 369–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wall, H.M., & Routowicz, A. (1987). Use of self-generated cues in immediate and delayed recall.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 64, 1019–1022.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, A.Y., & Thomas, M.H. (1992). The effects of imagery-based mnemonics on the long-term retention of Chinese characters.Language Learning, 42(3), 359–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, A.., & Thomas, M.H. (1996). Mnemonic instruction and gifted child.Roeper Review, 19(2), 104–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, A.Y., Thomas, M.H., & Quellette, J.A. (1992). Keyword mnemonic retention of second-language vocabulary words.Journal of Educational Psychology, 4, 520–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, G.C. (1989). Research on teaching Chinese in forty-five universities: Analysis of survey results.Journal of Chinese Language Teaching Association, 24(3) 101–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S. (1998). A study on the learning and teaching of Hanzi-Chinese characters.Working Papers in Educational Linguistics, 14(1), 69–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, M.D. (1996). Learner-control and instructional technologies. In D.H. Jonassen (Ed.),Handbook of research in educational communications and technology (pp. 957–983). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittrock, M.C. (1990). Generative processes of comprehension.Educational Psychologist, 24(4), 345–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Amy Kuo, ML., Hooper, S. The effects of visual and verbal coding mnemonics on learning Chinese characters in computer-based instruction. ETR&D 52, 23–34 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02504673

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02504673

Keywords

Navigation