Primates

, Volume 21, Issue 1, pp 100–110 | Cite as

Kidnapping and female competition among captive bonnet macaques

  • Joan B. Silk
Article

Abstract

Rank, parity, and kinship influence the pattern and frequency of kidnapping interactions in a group of captive bonnet macaques,Macaca radiata. Kidnapping attempts are initiated by parous and nulliparous females toward the infants of lower ranking females. Contrary to the pattern documented in several other species, nulliparous females participate in kidnapping interactions at the same rate as lactating females. Kidnapping attempts are consistently resisted by mothers, and most attempts are unsuccessful. Both the rates and pattern of kidnapping interactions differ within and between lineages. The results suggest that kidnappers gain neither status nor maternal experience and that the mothers of kidnapped infants do not benefit at all. Kidnapping may represent a form of competition among females which is detrimental to both infants and their mothers.

Keywords

Lactate Lower Ranking Animal Ecology Ranking Female Lactate Female 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alexander, B. K. &E. M. Roth, 1971. The effects of crowding on aggressive behavior of Japanese monkeys.Behaviour, 39: 73–90.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Alexander, R. D., 1974. The evolution of social behavior.Ann. Rev. Ecol. System., 5: 325–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barash, D. P., 1977.Sociobiology and Behavior. Elsevier, New York.Google Scholar
  4. Cheney, D. L., 1977. The acquisition of rank and the development of reciprocal alliances among free ranging baboons.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 2(3): 303–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dawkins, R., 1976.The Selfish Gene. Oxford Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  6. Eaton, G. G., 1976. The social order of Japanese macaques.Scient. Amer., 97: 106.Google Scholar
  7. Fedigan, L. M., 1976.A Study of Roles in the Arashiyama West Troop of Japanese Monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Contributions to Primatology, Vol. 8, Karger, Basel.Google Scholar
  8. Fossey, D., 1976. The behaviour of the mountain gorilla. Ph.D. dissertation, Cambridge Univ.Google Scholar
  9. Gartlan, J. S., 1969. Sexual and maternal behavior of the vervet monkey,Cercopithecus aethiops.J. Reprod. Fertil. (Suppl.), 6: 137–150.Google Scholar
  10. Goodall, J. van Lawick, 1968. The behavior of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Research Center.Anim. Behav. Monog., 1: 165–311.Google Scholar
  11. Hamilton, W. D., 1964. The genetical evolution of social behavior.J. Theoret. Biol., 7: 1–51.Google Scholar
  12. ————, 1975. Innate social aptitudes of man: an approach from evolutionary genetics. In:Biosocial Anthropology, R. Fox (ed.), Malaby, London, pp. 133–155.Google Scholar
  13. Hinde, R. A., 1974.The Biological Bases of Human Social Behavior. MacGraw Hill, New York.Google Scholar
  14. ---- &Y. Spencer-Booth, 1969. The effect of social companions on mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys. In:Primates Ethology,D. Morris (ed.), Aldine, Chicago, pp. 343–364.Google Scholar
  15. Hrdy, S. B., 1976. Care and exploitation of nonhuman primate infants by conspecifics other than the mother. In:Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol. 6,L. A. Rosenblum (ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 101–158.Google Scholar
  16. ————, 1977.The Langurs of Abu. Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  17. ----, in press. Matriarchs and altruists: the behavior of senescent females in macaque and langur sisterhoods. In:Other Ways of Growing Old,P. Amoss & S. Harell (eds.).Google Scholar
  18. Itani, J., 1959. Paternal care in a group of free ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Folia Primat., 20: 178–210.Google Scholar
  19. Kawai, M., 1958. On the system of social ranks in a natural troop of Japanese monkeys.Primates, 1: 84–89.Google Scholar
  20. Kawamura, S., 1958. Matriarchal social ranks in the Minoo-B troop: a study of the rank system of Japanese monkeys. In:Japanese Monkeys,S. A. Altmann (ed.), pp. 149–156 (Originally appeared inPrimates, 1: 149–156).Google Scholar
  21. Koyama, N., 1967. On dominance rank and kinship of a wild Japanese monkey troop in Arashiyama.Primates, 8: 189–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. ————, 1970. Changes in dominance rank and division of a wild Japanese monkey troop in Arashiyama.Primates, 11: 355–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kurland, J. A., 1977.Kin Selection in the Japanese Monkey. Contributions to Primatology, Vol. 12, Karger, Basel.Google Scholar
  24. Lancaster, J. B., 1971. Play-mothering: the relations between juvenile females and young infants among free-ranging vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops).Folia Primat., 15: 161–182.Google Scholar
  25. Loy, J., 1975. The effects of matrilineal relations on the behavior of juvenile rhesus monkeys.Abstracts of 71st Meet. Amer. Anthrop. Ass., pp. 153–180.Google Scholar
  26. Marsden, A. M., 1968. Agonistic behavior of young rhesus monkeys after changes induced in the social rank of their mothers.Anim. Behav., 16: 38–44.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Missakian, E. A., 1972. Genealogical and cross genealogical dominance relationships in a group of free ranging rhesus monkeys on Cayo Santiago.Primates, 13: 169–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nicholson, N. A., 1977. A comparison of early behavioral development in wild and captive chimpanzees. In:Primate Biosocial Development,S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff &F. E. Poirier (eds.), Garland, New York, pp. 529–562.Google Scholar
  29. Owens, N. W., 1975. Social play behavior in free-living baboons,Papio anubis. Anim. Behav., 23: 387–408.Google Scholar
  30. Packer, C., 1977. Reciprocal altruism in olive baboons.Nature, 255: 219–220.Google Scholar
  31. Pusey, A. E., 1977. The physical and social development of wild adolescent chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthi). Ph. D. dissertation, Stanford Univ.Google Scholar
  32. Rahaman, H. &M. D. Parsatharathy, 1962. Studies of the social behavior of bonnet monkeys.Primates, 10: 149–162.Google Scholar
  33. Ransom, T. W. &T. E. Rowell, 1972. Early social development of feral baboons. In:Primate Socialization,F. E. Poirier (ed.), Random House, New York, pp. 105–144.Google Scholar
  34. Sade, D. S., 1967. Determinants of dominance in a group of free-ranging rhesus monkeys. In:Social Communication in Primates,S. A. Altmann (ed.), Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 99–114.Google Scholar
  35. ————, 1972. A longitudinal study of social behavior of rhesus monkeys. In:The Functional and Evolutionary Biology of Primates,R. H. Tuttle (ed.), Aldine-Atherton, Chicago, pp. 378–398.Google Scholar
  36. Silk, J. B. &H. C. Kraemer, 1978. Proximity of mother and infant chimpanzees in captivity and in the wild. In:Recent Advances in Primatology, Vol. 1, Behavior,D. J. Chivers &P. Herbert (eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 115–118.Google Scholar
  37. Trivers, R. L., 1971. The evolution of reciprocal altruism.Quart. Rev. Biol., 46: 35–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wilson, E. O., 1975.Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  39. Yamada, M., 1963. A study of blood relationship in the natural society of the Japanese macaque.Primates, 4: 43–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre 1980

Authors and Affiliations

  • Joan B. Silk
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Anthropology and California Primate Research CenterUniversity of California, DavisDavisUSA

Personalised recommendations