Skip to main content
Log in

On the utility of aPost Hoc correction procedure for one-zero sampling duration estimates

  • Published:
Primates Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Numerous previous studies have shown that one-zero sampling in direct observation systematically overestimates behavioral durations. Apost hoc correction procedure has been proposed and demonstrated to be capable of removing most of the systematic errors found in one-zero duration estimates. However, the remaining errors for some individual observation sessions can still be quite substantial in practical terms. In this paper, the condition under which thepost hoc correction procedure will produce results with negligible systematic errors in one-zero duration estimates is identified.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Altmann, J., 1974. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods.Behaviour, 49: 227–267.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ary, D., 1984. Mathematical prediction of sampling component of behavior observation measurement error.Behav. Assess., 6: 221–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • ————, &H. K. Suen, 1983. The use of momentary time sampling to assess both frequency and duration of behavior.J. Behav. Assess., 5: 143–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brulle, A. R. &A. C. Repp, 1984. An investigation of the accuracy of momentary time sampling procedures with time series data.Brit. J. Psychol., 75: 481–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R., 1976. Some aspects of research design and their implications in the observational study of behavior.Behaviour, 58: 79–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodenough, F. L., 1928. Measuring behavior traits by means of repeated short samples.J. Juv. Res., 12: 230–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, S. B. &L. G. Alverson, 1978. A comparison of indirect measures of long-duration behaviors.J. Appl. Behav. Analys., 11: 530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, B. &R. Adams, 1983. A parametric model for estimating prevalence, incidence, and mean bout duration from point sampling.Amer. J. Primatol., 4: 261–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraemer, H. C., 1979. One-zero sampling in the study of primate behavior.Primates, 20: 237–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legar, D., 1977. An empirical evaluation of instantaneous and one-zero sampling of chimpanzee behavior.Primates, 18: 387–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDowell, E., 1973. Comparison of time-sampling and continuous recording techniques for observing developmental change in caretaker and infant behaviors.J. Genet. Psychol., 123: 99–105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milar, C. &R. Hawkins, 1976. Distorted results from the use of interval recording techniques. In:Behaviour Analysis in Education: Self-control and Reading,T. Bringham,J. Scott, &T. Laughling (eds.), Kendall/Hunt, Dubuque, Iowa, pp. 261–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G. &E. Goodall, 1980. Measurement error in direct observations: A comparison of common recording methods.Behav. Res. Therapy, 18: 147–150.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, J., A. Martindale, &S. Kulp, 1975. An evaluation of time-sample measure of behavior.J. Appl. Behav. Analys., 8: 463–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • ————,D. Martindale, S. Kulp, A. Martindale, &R. Bauman, 1977. Taking a closer look: Time sampling and measurement error.J. Appl. Behav. Analys., 10: 325–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • ————, &R. Rockinson, 1978. On the inability of interval time sampling to reflect frequency of occurrence data.J. Appl. Behav. Analys., 11: 531–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhine, R. J. &M. Flanigon, 1978. An empirical comparison of one-zero, focal-animal, and instantaneous methods of sampling spontaneous primate social behavior.Primates, 19: 353–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———— &A. K. Linville, 1980. Properties of one-zero scores in observational studies of primate social behavior: The effect of assumptions on empirical analyses.Primates, 21: 111–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanson-Fisher, R., A. Poole, &J. Dunn, 1980. An empirical method for determining an appropriate interval length for recording behavior.J. Appl. Behav. Analys., 13: 493–500.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, M. J. A. &A. E. Simpson, 1977. One-zero and scan-methods for sampling behavior.Anim. Behav., 25: 726–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suen, H. K. &D. Ary, 1984. Variables influencing one-zero and instantaneous time sampling outcomes.Primates, 25: 89–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———— & ————, 1986a. Poisson cumulative probabilities of systematic errors in single-subject and multiple-subject time sampling.Behav. Assess., 8: 155–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———— & ————, 1986b. Apost hoc correction procedure for systematic errors in time sampling duration estimates.J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess., 8(1): 31–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

About this article

Cite this article

Suen, H.K. On the utility of aPost Hoc correction procedure for one-zero sampling duration estimates. Primates 27, 237–244 (1986). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02382602

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02382602

Key Words

Navigation