Abstract
A total 202 social staring episodes (prolonged gazing by one individual toward another within a short distance) were observed in various social contexts among six unrelated, adult, and subadult male mountain gorillas. Staring was not accompanied by distinct facial expressions by actors or recipients, irrespective of their age or dominance rank. Younger, subordinate animals tended to stare at elder, dominant animals more frequently than vice versa. Staring may have multiple functions depending on the social context. In the initiation of non-agonistic interactions, staring rarely occurred, but was very successful for younger males in eliciting play or homosexual interactions from older animals. Staring was also directed by younger males to older males for greeting or appeasement. It may possibly play a role in reducing the increased social tension that occurs during or after conflict and in averting the potential conflict among older males. Younger males occasionally supplanted older males by staring at feeding spots. This slow supplantation is similar to the phenomenon of food sharing achieved through begging behavior in chimpanzees and bonobos. A prolonged gaze including staring by subordinates towards dominants may characterize the frequent and prolonged face-to-face interactions in the African great apes, and contrasts with a frequent gaze aversion by subordinates towards dominants in macaques or baboons. A difference between gorillas and other apes is that, chimpanzees and bonobos make positive contact with each other through eye contact, while gorillas simply stare at another without physical contact in greeting or appeasement process. Staring may serve an effective strategy for younger male gorillas to intervene safely in olders' conflict and sometimes to suppress or inhibit olders' performance, but their non-agonistic contacts or supporting attacks may not contribute to the establishment or support of social bonds between them. It is possible that staring may be common among the African great apes and man, and that it has evolved as a tactics to mask the dominant/subordinate relationships between individuals with multiple functions.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
References
Altmann, S. A., 1962. A field study of the sociobiology of rhesus monkeys,Macaca mulatta.Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 102: 338–435.
Bertrand, M., 1969.The Behavioral Repertoire of the Stumptail Macaque, Bibliotheca Primatologica, No. 11. Karger, Basel.
Caro, T. M., 1976. Observations on the ranging behaviour and daily activity of lone silverback mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei).Anim. Behav., 24: 889–897.
Chance, M. R. A., 1956. Social structure of a colony ofMacaca mulatta.Brit. J. Anim. Behav., 4: 1–13.
Cheney, D. L., 1977. The acquisition of rank and the development of reciprocal alliances among free-ranging immature baboons.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 2: 303–318.
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S., 1974. Male-female, female-female, and male-male sexual behavior in the stumptail monkey, with special attention to the female orgasm.Arc. Sex. Behav., 3(2): 95–116.
Datta, S. B., 1989. Demographic influences on dominance structure among female primates. In:Comparative Socioecology,V. Standen &R. A. Foley (eds.), Blackwell Scientific Publ., Oxford, pp. 265–284.
Dolhinow, P. J. &N. Bishop, 1970. The development of motor skills and social relationships among primates through play.Minn. Symp. Child Psychol., 4: 141–198.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I., 1973.Der Vorprogrammierte Mensch. Verlag Fritz Molden, Wien.
Elliott, R. C., 1976. Observation on a small group of mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei).Folia Primatol., 25: 12–24.
Enomoto, T., 1974. The sexual behavior of Japanese monkeys.J. Human Evol., 3: 351–372.
Fossey, D., 1972. Vocalization of the mountain gorilla (Gorilla gorilla beringei).Anim. Behav., 20: 36–53.
————, 1974. Observations on the home range of one group of mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei).Anim. Behav., 22: 568–581.
————, 1981. The imperiled mountain gorilla.Nat. Geogr., 159: 501–523.
————, 1982. Reproduction among free-living mountain gorillas.Amer. J. Primatol. Suppl., 1: 97–104.
————, 1983.Gorillas in the Mist. Houghton Mifflin, Boston.
———— &A. H. Harcourt, 1977. Feeding ecology of free-ranging mountain gorilla (Gorilla gorilla beringei). In:Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging Behaviour in Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes,T. H. Clutton-Brock (ed.), Academic Press, London, pp. 415–447.
Furuichi, T., 1989. Social interactions and the life history of femalePan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire.Int. J. Primatol., 10: 173–197.
Goodall, J., 1965. Chimpanzees of the Gombe Stream Reserve. In:Primate Behavior,I. DeVore (ed.), Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, New York, pp. 425–473.
————, 1968. The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve.Anim. Behav. Monogr., 1(3): 161–311.
Hall, E. T., 1966.The Hidden Dimension. Doubleday, Garden City, New York.
Hall, K. R. L. &I. DeVore, 1965. Baboon social behavior. In:Primate Behavior,I. DeVore (ed.), Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, New York, pp. 53–110.
———— &B. Mayer, 1967. Social interactions in a group of captive patas monkey (Erythrocebus patas).Folia Primatol., 5: 213–236.
Harcourt, A. H., 1978. Strategies of emigration and transfer by primates, with particular reference to gorillas.Z. Tierpsychol., 48: 401–420.
————, 1979a. Social relationships among adult female mountain gorillas.Anim. Behav., 27: 251–264.
————, 1979b. Social relationships between adult male and female mountain gorillas in the wild.Anim. Behav., 27: 325–342.
———— & K. J. Stewart, 1987. The influence of help in contests on dominance rank in primates: hints from gorillas.Anim. Behav., 35: 182–190.
———— & ————, 1989. Functions of alliances in contests within wild gorilla groups.Behaviour, 109: 176–190.
————, ————, &D. Fossey, 1976. Male emigration and female transfer in wild mountain gorilla.Nature, 263: 226–227.
————, ————, & ————, 1981. Gorilla reproduction in the wild. In:Reproductive Biology of the Great Apes,E. E. Graham (ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 265–279.
Hediger, H., 1955.Studies of the Psychology and Behaviour of Captive Animals in Zoos and Circuses. Criterion Books, New York.
Idani, G., 1991. Relations between unit-groups of bonobos at Wamba, Zaire: encounters and temporary fusions.Afr. Stud. Monogr., 11(3): 153–186.
Itani, J., 1954.Nihon Dobutsuki II, The Japanese Monkeys at Takasakiyama.K. Imanishi (ed.), Kobunsha, Tokyo. (in Japanese).
Kano, T., 1980. Social behavior of wild pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba: a preliminary report.J. Human Evol., 9: 243–260.
Kitamura, K., 1989. Genito-genital contacts in the pygmy chimpanzee (Pan paniscus).Afr. Stud. Monogr., 10(2): 49–67.
Koyama, N., 1985. Playmate relationships among individuals of a Japanese monkey troop in Arashiyama.Primates, 26: 390–406.
Kummer, H., 1967. Tripartite relations in hamadryas baboons. In:Social Communication Among Primates,S. A. Altmann (ed.), Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 63–71.
Kurland, J. A., 1977.Kin Selection in the Japanese Monkey. Contributions to Primatology, Vol. 12, S. Karger, Basel.
Kuroda, S., 1980. Social behavior of the pygmy chimpanzees.Primates, 21: 181–197.
————, 1984. Interaction over food among pygmy chimpanzees. In:The Pygmy Chimpanzees: Evolutionary Biology and Behavior,R. L. Susman (ed.), Plenum Press, New York & London, pp. 301–324.
Levy, J., 1979. Play behavior and its decline during development in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Ph. D. diss., Univ. of Chicago.
Loizos, C., 1966. Play in mammals. In:Play, Exploration, and Territory in Mammals,J. A. Jewell (ed.), Academic Press, London, pp. 1–19.
————, 1977. Play behaviour in higher primates: a review. In:Primate Ethology,D. Morris (ed.), Aldine, Chicago, pp. 226–282.
Morgan, B. J. T., M. J. A. Simpson, J. P. Hanby, &J. Hall-Craggs, 1976. Visualizing interaction and sequential data in animal behavior: theory and application of cluster-analysis methods.Behaviour, 59: 1–43.
Nishida, T., 1970. social behavior and relationship among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali mountains.Primates, 11: 47–87.
————, 1983. Alpha status and agonistic alliance in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii).Primates, 24: 318–336.
———— &M. Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, 1987. Chimpanzees and bonobos: cooperative relationships among males. In:Primate Societies,B. B. Smuts,D. L. Cheney,R. M. Seyfarth,R. W. Wrangham, andT. T. Struhsaker (eds.), Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 165–177.
Oki, J. &Y. Maeda, 1973. Grooming as a regulator of behavior in Japanese macaques. In:Behavioral Regulators of Behavior in Primates,C. R. Carpenter (ed.), Bucknell Univ. Press, Lewisburg, pp. 149–163.
Packer, C. &A. Pusey, 1979. Female aggression and male membership in troops of Japanese macaques and olive baboons.Folia Primatol., 31: 212–218.
Redican, W. K., 1975. Facial expressions in nonhuman primates. In:Primate Behavior,L. A. Rosenblum (ed.), Academic Press, New York, San Francisco, & London, pp. 103–194.
Sade, D. S., 1965. Some aspects of parent-offspring and sibling relations in a group of rhesus monkeys, with a discussion of grooming.Amer. J. Phys. Anthropol., 23: 1–18.
Savage, E. S. &R. Bakeman, 1978. Sexual morphology and behavior inPan paniscus. In:Recent Advances in Primatology,R. Chivers &J. Herbert (eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 613–616.
Schaller, G. B., 1963.The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Silk, J. B., 1982. Altruism among femaleMacaca radiata: explanations and analysis of patterns of grooming and coalition formation.Behaviour, 79: 162–188.
Struhsaker, T. T., 1967. Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In:Social Communication Among Primates,S. A. Altmann (ed.), Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 281–324.
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., 1962. Facial expressions in higher primates.Symp. Zool. Soc. London, 8: 97–125.
————, 1969. The facial displays of the catarrhine monkeys and apes. In:Primate Ethology,D. Morris (ed.), Anchor Doubleday, Garden City, New York, pp. 9–88.
de Waal, F. B. M. &R. M. Ren, 1988. Comparison of the reconciliation behavior of stumptail and rhesus macaques.Ethology, 78: 129–142.
———— &A. van Roosmalen, 1979. Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 5: 55–66.
———— &D. Yoshihara, 1983. Reconciliation and redirected affection in rhesus monkeys.Behaviour, 85: 224–241.
Watanabe, K., 1979. Alliance formation in a free-ranging troop of Japanese macaques.Primates, 20: 459–474.
Watts, D. P., 1984. Composition and variability of mountain gorilla diets in the central Virungas.Amer. J. Primatol., 7: 323–356.
————, 1985. Relations between group size and composition and feeding competition in mountain gorilla groups.Anim. Behav., 33: 72–85.
Wrangham, R. W., 1975.The Behavioral Ecology of Chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Ph. D. diss., Univ. of Cambridge, Cambridge.
Yamagiwa, J., 1986. Activity rhythm and the ranging of a solitary male mountain gorilla (Gorilla gorilla beringei).Primates, 27: 273–282.
————, 1987a. Intra- and inter-group interactions of an all-male group of Virunga mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei).Primates, 28: 1–30.
————, 1987b. Male life history and the social structure of wild mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei). In:Evolution and Coadaptation in Biotic Communities,S. Kawano,J. H. Connell, &T. Hidaka (eds.), Univ. of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 31–51.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
About this article
Cite this article
Yamagiwa, J. Functional analysis of social staring behavior in an all-male group of mountain gorillas. Primates 33, 523–544 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381153
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381153