Advertisement

Primates

, Volume 31, Issue 2, pp 225–241 | Cite as

Sexual preferences during artificial menstrual cycles in social groups of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)

  • Richard P. Michael
  • Doris Zumpe
Article

Abstract

Eight groups of rhesus monkeys each consisting of one male and four ovariectomized females were observed while two of the females were treated with hormones to produce artificial menstrual cycles. These were either synchronized or offset by 7-day increments. Sexually preferred females, defined by the numbers of ejaculations per test, received almost twice as many ejaculations as did non-preferred females during all synchronized and offset cycles and during all cycle phases. However, short-term changes in partner preference occurred when the midcycle phase of non-preferred females coincided with the middle or late progesterone phase of preferred females, suggesting a negative effect of progesterone on behavior during the menstrual cycle. There were highly significant differences between preferred and non-preferred partners for almost all of their sexual and social interactions, and preferred partners showed longer proximity and grooming times as well as higher levels of sexual activity. Partner preferences accounted for more of the behavioral variance between pairs than did female dominance, although males sought the proximity of dominant females independently of their partner preferences. Thus, in a setting uncomplicated by male mate competition, sexual preference by male rhesus monkeys is a robust phenomenon depending on complex interactions between dominance, hormonal status, and the individual behavior of female partners.

Key Words

Macaca mulatta Hormone cycles Behavioral rhythms Partner preferences Female dominance Mate choice Mate competition 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bonsall, R. W., D. Zumpe, &R. P. Michael, 1978. Menstrual cycle influences on operant behavior of female rhesus monkeys.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 92: 846–855.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Chambers, K. C. &C. H. Phoenix, 1984. Restoration of sexual performance in old rhesus macaques paired with a preferred female partner.Int. J. Primatol., 5: 287–298.Google Scholar
  3. Chapais, B. &F. Larose, 1988. Experimental rank reversals among peers inMacaca fuscata: rank is maintained after the removal of kin support.Amer. J. Primatol., 16: 1–42.Google Scholar
  4. Ehardt, C. L. &I. S. Bernstein, 1986. Matrilineal overthrows in rhesus monkey groups.Int. J. Primatol., 7: 157–181.Google Scholar
  5. Everitt, B. J. &J. Herbert, 1969. The role of ovarian hormones in the sexual preference of rhesus monkeys.Anim. Behav., 17: 738–746.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Hill, D. A., 1987. Social relationships between adult male and female rhesus macaques: 1. Sexual consortships.Primates, 28: 439–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Michael, R. P., R. W. Bonsall, &D. Zumpe, 1976. Evidence for chemical communication in primates.Vitam. Horm., 34: 137–186.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. ————, ————, & ————, 1978. Consort bonding and operant behavior by female rhesus monkeys.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 92: 837–845.Google Scholar
  9. ———— &G. S. Saayman, 1967. Individual differences in the sexual behaviour of male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) under laboratory conditions.Anim. Behav., 15: 460–466.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. ———— &D. Zumpe, 1970. Aggression and gonadal hormones in captive rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Anim. Behav., 18: 1–10.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. ———— & ————, 1982. Influence of olfactory signals on the reproductive behaviour of social groups of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).J. Endocrinol., 95: 189–205.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. ———— & ————, 1988. Determinants of behavioral rhythmicity during artificial menstrual cycles in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Amer. J. Primatol., 15: 157–170.Google Scholar
  13. ————, ————, &R. W. Bonsall, 1982. Behavior of rhesus monkeys during artificial menstrual cycles.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 96: 875–885.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Missakian, E. A., 1972. Genealogical and cross-genealogical dominance relations in a group of free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) on Cayo Santiago.Primates, 13: 169–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. ————, 1973. Genealogical mating activity in free-ranging groups of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) on Cayo Santiago.Behaviour, 45: 225–241.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Packer, C., 1979. Inter-troop transfer and inbreeding avoidance inPapio anubis.Anim. Behav., 27: 1–36.Google Scholar
  17. Phoenix, C. H., 1973. Ejaculation by male rhesus as a function of the female partner.Horm. Behav., 4: 365–370.Google Scholar
  18. Sade, D. S., 1968. Inhibition of son-mother mating among free-ranging rhesus monkeys.Sci. Psychoanal., 12: 18–38.Google Scholar
  19. Smith, D. G., 1987. Generational bias in mating in captive groups of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Folia Primatol., 49: 212–215.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. ———— &M. F. Small, 1987. Mate choice by lineage in three captive groups of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).Amer. J. Phys. Anthropol., 73: 185–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Takahata, Y., 1982. The sociosexual behavior of Japanese monkeys.Z. Tierpsychol., 59: 89–108.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Wallen, K., L. A. Winston, S. Gaventa, M. Davis-Dasilva, &D. C. Collins, 1984. Periovulatory changes in female sexual behavior and patterns of ovarian steroid secretion in group-living rhesus monkeys.Horm. Behav., 18: 431–450.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Wilson, M. E. &T. P. Gordon, 1979. Age differences in the duration of mating periods of female rhesus monkeys.Develop. Psychobiol., 13: 637–642.Google Scholar
  24. ————, ————, &D. C. Collins, 1982. Serum 17β-estradiol and progesterone associated with mating behavior during early pregnancy in female rhesus monkeys.Horm. Behav., 16: 94–106.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Winer, B. J., 1971.Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. McGraw-Hill Book Co., New York.Google Scholar
  26. Zumpe, D. &R. P. Michael, 1970. Redirected aggression and gonadal hormones in captive rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Anim. Behav., 18: 11–19.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. ———— & ————, 1979. Relation between the hormonal status of the female and direct and redirected aggression by male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Horm. Behav., 12: 269–279.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. ———— & ————, 1984. Low potency of intact male rhesus monkeys after long-term visual contact with their female partners.Amer. J. Primatol., 6: 241–252.Google Scholar
  29. ———— & ————, 1986. Dominance index: A simple measure of relative dominance status in primates.Amer. J. Primatol., 10: 291–300.Google Scholar
  30. ———— & ————, 1987. Relation between the dominance rank of female rhesus monkeys and their access to males.Amer. J. Primatol., 13: 155–169.Google Scholar
  31. ———— & ————, 1989. Female dominance rank and behavior during artificial menstrual cycles in social groups of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Amer. J. Primatol., 17: 287–304.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Japan Monkey Centre 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Richard P. Michael
    • 1
  • Doris Zumpe
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychiatryEmory University School of Medicine, The Georgia Mental Health InstituteAtlantaU.S.A.

Personalised recommendations