Learner preferences and achievement under differing amounts of learner practice

  • Heidi L. Schnackenberg
  • Howard J. Sullivan
  • Lars F. Leader
  • Elizabeth E. K. Jones
Research

Abstract

This study examined the effects of program mode (i.e., a lean program version containing a basic amount of learner practice vs. a full mode containing expanded practice) and learner preference (matched or unmatched) for amount of practice on the achievement, time-in-program, and attitudes of university undergraduate students. Subjects completed a 10-item Likert-type prequestionnaire to indicate the amount of practice they preferred, then were randomly assigned to either the type of program they preferred or to the opposite type. Subjects who used the full version of the instructional program scored significantly higher on the posttest than those who used the lean version. Matching subjects to their preferred amount of practice did not yield a significant achievement difference over assigning subjects to their less-preferred amount. Subjects preferred the lean version of the program over the full one, even though the full version produced better test performance.

Keywords

Undergraduate Student Educational Technology Good Test Program Mode Program Version 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Atkinson, R.C. (1972). Ingredients for a theory of instruction.American Psychologist, 27, 921–931.Google Scholar
  2. Bower, G.H., Thompson-Schill, S., & Tulving, E. (1994). Reducing retroactive interference: and interference analysis.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 20(1), 51–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Campbell, C.C. (1993). Accessing related events increases retroactive interference in a matching recognition test.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 19(4), 967–974.Google Scholar
  4. Carrier, C.A. (1984). Do learners make good choices?Instructional Innovator, 29(2), 15–17, 48.Google Scholar
  5. Carrier, C.A., & Williams, M.D. (1988). A test of one learner control strategy with students of differing levels of task persistence.Education Research Journal, 25, 285–306.Google Scholar
  6. Freitag, E.T., & Sullivan, H.J. (1995). Matching learner preference for amount of instruction: An alternative form of learner control.Educational Technology Research and Development, 43(2), 5–14.Google Scholar
  7. Gagné, R.M. (1985).Conditions of Learning and Theory of Instruction (4th ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Google Scholar
  8. Gray, S.H. (1987). The effect of sequence control on computer assisted learning.Journal of Computer-Based Instruction, 14(2), 54–56.Google Scholar
  9. Hannafin, R.D., & Sullivan, H.J. (1995). Learner control in full and lean CAI programs.Educational Technology Research and Development, 43(1), 19–30.Google Scholar
  10. Hannafin, R.D., & Sullivan, H.J. (1996). Preferences and learner control over amount of instruction.Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 162–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hicken, S., Sullivan, H., & Klein, J.D. (1992). Learner control modes and Incentive variation in computer-delivered instruction.Educational Technology Research and Development, 40(4), 15–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hintze, H., Mohr, H., & Wenzel, A. (1988). Students' attitudes towards control methods in computer-assisted instruction.Journal of Computer-Assisted Learning, 4(1), 3–10.Google Scholar
  13. Igoe, A.R. (1993).Learner control over instruction and achievement goals in computer-assisted instruction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, Tempe.Google Scholar
  14. Jonassen, D.H. (1991). Objectivism versus constructivism: Do we need a new philosophical paradigm?Educational Technology Research and Development, 39(3), 5–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Jones, E.E.K., Sullivan, H. J., & Klein, J. D. (1996).Matching preferences for cooperative and individual learning with instructional treatments. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Educational Communications and Technology, Indianapolis, IN, 1996.Google Scholar
  16. Kinzie, M.B. (1990). Requirements and benefits of effective interactive instruction: Learner control, self-regulation, and continuing motivation.Educational Technology Research and Development, 38(1), 1–21.Google Scholar
  17. Kinzie, M.B., & Sullivan, H.J. (1989). Continuing motivation, learner control, and CAI.Educational Technology Research and Development, 37(2), 5–14.Google Scholar
  18. Kinzie, M.B., Sullivan, H.J., & Berdel, R.L. (1988). Learner control and achievement in science computer-assisted instruction.Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 299–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Mager, R.F. (1964). Learner-controlled instruction—1958–1964.Programmed Instruction, 4(2), 1, 8, 10–12.Google Scholar
  20. Mattoon, J.S. (1994).Instructional control in part- and whole-task training. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, Tempe.Google Scholar
  21. Merrill, M.D. (1975). Learner control: Beyond aptitude-treatment interactions.AV Communications Review, 23, 217–226.Google Scholar
  22. Merrill, M.D. (1980). Learner control in computer based learning.Computers and Education, 4, 77–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Pollock, J.C., & Sullivan, H.J. (1990). Practice mode and learner control in computer based instruction.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 15, 251–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Popham, W.J. (1969). Objectives and instruction. In W. J. Popham (Ed.),Instructional Objectives. Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
  25. Ross, S.M., Morrison, G.R., & O'Dell, J.K. (1988). Obtaining more out of less text in CBI: Effects of varied text density levels as a function of learner characteristics and control strategy.Educational Communication and Technology Journal, 36, 131–142.Google Scholar
  26. Ross, S.M., & Rakow, E.A. (1981). Learner control versus program control as adaptive strategies for selection of instructional support on math rules.Journal of Educational Psychology, 73(5).Google Scholar
  27. Shute, V.J. & Gluck, K.A. (1996). Individual differences in patterns of spontaneous online tool use.The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 5(4), 329–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Snow, R.E. (1980). Aptitude, learner control, and adaptive instruction.Educational Psychologist, 15, 151–158.Google Scholar
  29. Snow, R.E. & Peterson, P.L. (1980). Recognizing differences in student aptitudes. In W.J. McKeachie (Ed.),New Directions for Teaching and Learning, Cognition, and College Teaching No.2, (pp. 1–23). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  30. Snyder, T. & Sullivan, H. (1995). Cooperative and individual learning and student misconceptions in science.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 230–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Sullivan, H., & Higgins, N. (1983).Teaching for Competence. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© the Association for Educational Communications and Technology 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • Heidi L. Schnackenberg
    • 1
  • Howard J. Sullivan
    • 2
  • Lars F. Leader
    • 2
  • Elizabeth E. K. Jones
    • 2
  1. 1.the Concordia University Department of Education in QuebecCanada
  2. 2.the Department of Psychology in Education at Arizona State UniversityUSA

Personalised recommendations