Skip to main content
Log in

Diagrams as aids to problem solving: Their role in facilitating search and computation

  • Research
  • Published:
Educational Technology Research and Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In two experiments, university students solved kinship problems as they studied family trees or lists of statements about who was whose parent. Subjects were given problems to solve requiring the application of rules to the statements or trees. In the first experiment, response latency was less for subjects seeing trees than for those seeing statements. It was also found that latency increased as the problems became more difficult. In the second experiment, response latencies for subjects seeing no rules were less on difficult problems than for subjects working with rules involving English and nonsense kinship terms. This difference disappeared with practice. These findings provided support for an account of diagram interpretation based on the facilitation of search and computation through the spatial arrangement of concepts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Armbruster, B. B., & Anderson, T. H. (1982).Idea mapping: The technique and its use in the classroom, or simulating the “ups” and “downs” of reading comprehension. Reading Education Report #36. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Center for the Study of Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armbruster, B. B., & Anderson, T. H. (1984). Mapping: Representing informative text graphically. In C. D. Holley & D. F. Dansereau (Eds.),Spatial learning strategies. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrier, C., Post, T. R., & Heck, W. (1985). Using microcomputers with fourth-grade students to reinforce arithmetic skills.Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 16, 45–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeKleer, J., & Brown, J. S. (1981). Mental models of physical mechanisms and their acquisition. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.),Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Polson, P. G. (1988). A cognitive analysis of exceptional memory for restaurant orders. In M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. J. Farr (Eds.),The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data.Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1989). Situations, mental models, and generative knowledge. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.),Complex information processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guri-Rozenblit, S. (1988a). Impact of diagrams on recalling sequential elements in expository texts.Reading Psychology, 9, 121–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guri-Rozenblit, S. (1988b). The interrelationships between diagrammatic representations and verbal explanations in learning from social science texts.Instructional Science, 17, 219–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holliday, W. G., Brunner, L. L., & Donais, E. L. (1977). Differential cognitive and affective responses to flow diagrams in science.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 14, 129–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, R. E. (1988).Radial outlining: An instructional tool for teaching information processing. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Washington, Seattle, WA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D., Pittelman, S. D., & Heimlich, J. E. (1986). Semantic mapping.The Reading Teacher, 39, 778–783.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D., & Hawk, P. (1984). Using graphic organizers in instruction.Information Design Journal, 4, 58–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirk, R. E. (1968).Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H. (1989). Display-based problem solving. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.),Complex information processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words.Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milliken, G. A., & Johnson, D. E. (1984).Analysis of messy data, Volume 1: Designed experiments. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. W., & Readance, J. E. (1984). A quantitative and qualitative review of graphic organizer research.Journal of Educational Research, 78, 11–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R. (1981). Perceptual organization in information processing. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.),Perceptual organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R., Pristach, E. A., & Carson, C. E. (1989). Attention and object perception. In B. E. Shepp & S. Ballesteros (Eds.),Object perception: Structure and process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R., & Schwaitzberg, S. D. (1975). Grouping by proximity: Selective attention measures.Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 355–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I. (1986). The description and analysis of object and event perception. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.),The handbook of perception and human performance, Volume 2.

  • Schwartz, R. M., & Raphael, T. E. (1985). Concept of definition: A key to improving students' vocabulary.The Reading Teacher, 39, 198–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1989). The scientist as problem solver. In D. Klahr & K. Kotovsky (Eds.),Complex information processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in chess.American Scientist, 61, 394–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1938).Laws of organization in perceptual forms in a source book for Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winn, W. D. (1980). The effect of block-word diagrams on the structuring of concepts as a function of general ability.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 17, 201–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winn, W. D., & Sutherland, S. W. (1989). Factors influencing the recall of elements in maps and diagrams and the strategies used to encode them.Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 33–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Data from the first experiment reported here were presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA, March 1989.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Winn, W., Li, TZ. & Schill, D. Diagrams as aids to problem solving: Their role in facilitating search and computation. ETR&D 39, 17–29 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02298104

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02298104

Keywords

Navigation