Ahrens, J., & Dieter, U. (1974). Computer methods for sampling from gamma, beta, poisson and binomial distributions.Computing, 12, 223–246.
Google Scholar
Ahrens, J., & Dieter, U. (1982). Generating gamma variates by a modified rejection technique.Communications of the Association for Computing Machinery (ACM), 25, 47–54.
Google Scholar
Andrews, S., & Heathcote, A. (2001). Distinguishing common and task-specific processes in word identification: A matter of some moment.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 514–544.
Article
Google Scholar
Ashby, F. G., Tien, J.-Y., & Balikrishnan, J.D. (1993). Response time distributions in memory scanning.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 37, 526–555.
Google Scholar
Ashby, F. G., & Townsend, J.T. (1980). Decomposing the reaction time distribution: Pure insertion and selective influences revisited.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 21, 93–123.
Article
Google Scholar
Balota, D.A., & Chumbley, J.I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 340–357.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Balota, D.A., & Spieler, D.H. (1999). Word frequency, repetition, and lexicality effects in word recognition tasks: Beyond measures of central tendency.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 32–55.
Article
Google Scholar
Berger, J.O., & Sun, D. (1993). Bayesian analysis for the poly-Weibull distribution.Journal of the American Statistical Association, 88, 1412–1418.
Google Scholar
Bowman, K.O., & Shenton, L.R. (1988).Properties of estimators for the gamma distribution. New York, NY: Marcel Dekker.
Google Scholar
Busemeyer, J.R., & Townsend, J.T. (1993). Decision field theory: A dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment.Psychological Review, 100, 432–459.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cave, C., & Squire, L. (1992). Intact and long-lasting repetition priming in amnesia.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 509–520.
Article
Google Scholar
Dey, D., Ghosh, S., & Mallick, B. (2000).Generalized linear models: A Bayesian perspective. New York, NY: Marcel Dekker.
Google Scholar
Dolan, C., van der Maas, H., & Mollenaar, C. M. (in press). A framework for ML estimation of parameters of (mixtures of) common reaction time distributions given optional truncation or censoring.Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers.
Dzhafarov, E.N. (1992). The structure of simple reaction time to step-function signals.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 36, 235–268.
Google Scholar
Fox, J.P., & Glas, C.A.W. (2001). Bayesian estimation of a multilevel IRT model using Gibbs sampling.Psychometrika, 66, 271–288.
Article
Google Scholar
Gelfand, A., & Smith, A.F.M. (1990). Sampling based approaches to calculating marginal densities.Journal of the American Statistical Association, 85, 398–409.
Google Scholar
Gelman, A., Carlin, J.B., Stern, H.S., & Rubin, D.B. (1995).Bayesian data analysis. London, U.K.: Chapman and Hall.
Google Scholar
Gelman, A., & Rubin, D.B. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences (with discussion).Statistical Science, 7, 457–511.
Google Scholar
Gilks, W. & Wild, P. (1992). Adaptive rejection sampling for Gibbs sampling.Applied Statistics, 41, 337–348.
Google Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R.T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.
Google Scholar
Heathcote, A. (1996). RTSYS: A DOS application for the analysis of reaction time data.Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 28, 427–445.
Google Scholar
Heathcote, A., Popiel, S. J., & Mewhort, D. J. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: An example using the Stroop task.Psychological Bulletin, 109, 340–347.
Article
Google Scholar
Hockley, W.E. (1984). Analysis of reaction time distributions in the study of cognitive processes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 10, 598–615.
Google Scholar
Hohle, R.H. (1965). Inferred components of reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 382–386.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hsu, Y.F. (1999).Two studies on simple reaction times: I. On the psychophysics of the generalized Pieron's law. II. On estimating minimum detection times using the time estimation paradigm. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Irvine, CA: University of California.
Google Scholar
Jacoby, L.L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic fromm intentional uses of memory.Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541.
Article
Google Scholar
Jiang, Y., Rouder, J.N., & Speckman, P.L. (in press). A note on the sampling properties of the Vincentizing (quantile averaging) procedure.Journal of Mathematical Psychology.
Johnson, N.L., Kotz, S., & Balakrishnan, N. (1994).Continuous univariate distributions, Volume 2 (2nd ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Wiley.
Google Scholar
Kreft, I.G.G., & de Leeuew, J. (1998).Introducing multilevel modeling. London, U.K.: Sage.
Google Scholar
Link, S.W. (1975). The relative judgement theory of two choice response time.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 12, 114–135.
Article
Google Scholar
Logan, G.D. (1988). Towards an instance theory of automization.Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.
Article
Google Scholar
Logan, G.D. (1992). Shapes of reaction time distributions and shapes of learning curves: A test of the instance theory of automaticity.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 883–914.
Article
Google Scholar
Luce, R.D. (1986).Response times. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Madden, D.J., Gottlob, L.R., Denny, L.L., Turkington, T.G., Provenzale, J.M., Hawk, T.C., & Coleman, R.E. (1999). Aging and recognition memory: Changes in regional cerebral blood flow associated with components of reaction time distributions.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 511–520.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mitchell, D. & Brown, A. (1988). Persistent repetition priming in picture naming and its dissociation from recognition memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 14, 213–222.
Article
Google Scholar
Nelder, J.A., & Mead, R. (1965). A simplex method for function minimization.Computer Journal, 7, 308–313.
Google Scholar
Petrusic, W.M., Baranski, J.V., & Kennedy, R. (1999). Similarity comparisons with remembered and perceived magnitudes: Memory psychophysics and fundamental measurement.Memory & Cognition, 26, 1041–1055.
Google Scholar
Plourde, C.E., & Besner, D. (1997). On the locus of the word frequency effect in visual word recognition.Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 181–194.
Google Scholar
Posner, M.I. (1978).Chronometric explorations of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Google Scholar
Press, W.H., Teukolsky, S.A., Vetterling, W.T., & Flannery, F.P. (1992).Numerical recipes in C: The art of scientific computing (2nd ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval.Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.
Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics.Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446–461.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., & Rouder, J.N. (1998). Modeling response times for decisions between two choices.Psychological Science, 9, 347–356.
Article
Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., & Rouder, J. (2000). A diffusion model analysis of letter masking.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 127–140.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rouder, J.N. (1996). Premature sampling in random walks.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 40, 287–296.
Article
Google Scholar
Rouder, J.N. (2000). Assessing the roles of change discrimination and luminance integration: Evidence for a hybrid race model of perceptual decision making in luminance discrimination.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 359–378.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rouder, J.N. (2001). Testing evidence accrual models by manipulating stimulus onset.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 45, 334–354.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Schnieder, W., & Shiffrin, R.M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention.Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.
Google Scholar
Smith, P.L. (1995). Multiple detector models of visual simple reaction time.Psychological Review, 102, 567–593.
Article
Google Scholar
Smith, R.L., & Naylor, J.C. (1987). A comparison of maximum likelihood and Bayesian estimators for the three-parameter Weibull distribution.Applied Statistician, 36, 358–369.
Google Scholar
Spieler, D.H., Balota, D.A., & Faust, M.E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 461–479.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Spieler, D.H., Balota, D.A., & Faust, M.E. (2000). Levels of selective attention revealed through analyses of response time distributions.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 506–526.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory.Scuebce, 153, 652–654.
Google Scholar
Tanner, M.A. (1993).Tools for statistical inference: Methods for the exploration of posterior distributions and likelihood functions. Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Google Scholar
Thomas, E.A.C., & Ross, B. (1980). On appropriate procedures for combining probability distributions within the same family.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 21, 136–152.
Article
Google Scholar
Townsend, J.T., & Ashby, F.G. (1983).Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Treisman, A.M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention.Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ulrich, R., & Miller, J.O. (1993). Information processing models generating lognormally distributed reaction times.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 37, 513–525.
Article
Google Scholar
Van Zandt, T. (2000). How to fit a response time distribution.Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 424–465.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Van Zandt, T., Colonius, H., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). A comparison of two response time models applied to perceptual matching.Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 208–256.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Vincent, S.B. (1912). The function of vibrissae in the behavior of the white rat.Behavioral Monographs, 1, No. 5.
Wang, X.H., Bradlow, E.T., & Wainer, H. (2002). A general Bayesian model for testlets: Theory and applications.Applied Psychological Measurement, 26, 109–128.
Article
Google Scholar
Wild, P., & Gilks, W.R. (1993). Adaptive rejection sampling from log-concave density functions.Applied Statistics, 42, 701–708.
Google Scholar
Wixted, J.T., & Rohrer, D. (1993). Proactive interference and the dynamics of free recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 1024–1039.
Article
Google Scholar