Skip to main content
Log in

Noncognitive factors in high-road/low-road learning: I. Modes of abstraction in adulthood

  • Published:
Journal of Adult Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Since the termscognition andcognitive are broadly used but not clearly defined, it may be helpful to clarify what is meant bynoncognitive factors. In cognitive science, the termscognition andcognitive generally describe mental processes that are informational insofar as they carry information about the organism's own body and the material world. Thus defined, there are three sorts of noncognitive organismic factors important in adult learning:affective processes, self-developmental processes, andhardware factors (i.e., noninformational, purely organismic constraints such as mental capacity/working memory limitations, gestaltist field factors, etc.). In this series of papers, we attempt to show how these noncognitive factors interact with cognitive factors to facilitate adult learning. We outline and give reference to a dialectical constructivist (neoPiagetian) model of the psychological organism that integrates noncognitive with cognitive factors and that can serve to explicate the findings of the literature and to process/task analyze adult learning. An important aim is the integration of the findings of decline and regression from the cognitive literature with the findings regarding the increase in “self-directedness” reported by adult education theorists. This is explicated through a process-analytic account of the will, particularly as it pertains to noncognitive factors. In Part II, we continue our explication of a dialectical model of the ego and conclude with a discussion of modes of learning/instruction in adulthood.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983).The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1985).Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., Dittmann-Kohli, F., & Dixon, R. A. (1984). New perspectives on the development of intelligence in adulthood: Toward a dual-process conception and a model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.),Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 33–76). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basseches, M. (1984).Dialectical thinking and adult development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C. (1985). Toward a solution of the learning paradox.Review of Educational Research, 55(2), 201–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchard-Fields, F. (1989). Controllability and adaptive coping in the elderly: An adult developmental perspective. In P. S. Fry (Ed.),Psychological perspectives of helplessness and control in the elderly (pp. 43–61). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucouvalas, M., & Krupp, J. (1989). Adult development and learning. In S. B. Merriam & P. M. Cunningham (Eds.),Handbook of adult and continuing education (pp. 183–200). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B. (1992, May).On the evolution of consciousness and language. Psycoloquy.92.consciousness.1.bridgeman. Harnad@clarity.Princeton.EDU.

  • Brockett, R. G. (1985). Methodological and substantive issues in the measurement of self-directed learning readiness.Adult Education Quarterly, 36(1), 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brookfield, S. D. (1991).Understanding and facilitating adult learning: A comprehensive analysis of principles and effective practices. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S., & VanLehn, K. (1980). Repair theory: A generative theory of bugs in procedure skills.Cognitive Science, 4, 379–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caffarella, R. S., & Caffarella, E. P. (1986). Self-directedness and learning contracts in adult education.Adult Education Quarterly, 36(4), 226–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caffarella, R. S., & O'Donnell, J. M. (1987). Self-directed adult learning: A critical paradigm revisited.Adult Education Quarterly, 37(4), 199–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992).The mind's staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children's thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavanaugh, J., Grady, J., & Perlmutter, M. (1983). Forgetting and the use of memory aids in 20- to 70-year-olds' everyday life.International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 17, 113–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M. (1986). A functional account of age differences in memory. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.),Human memory and cognitive capabilities: Mechanisms and performances (pp. 409–422). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darkenwald, G. G., & Merriam, S. B. (1982).Adult education: Foundations of practice. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deshler, D., & Hagan, N. (1989). Adult educational research: Issues and directions. In S. B. Merriam & P. M. Cunningham (Eds.),Handbook of adult and continuing education (pp. 147–167). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, L. (1989). An investigation into the structure, validity, and reliability of Guglielmino's Self-directed Learning Readiness Scale.Adult Education Quarterly, 39(3), 125–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, R. (1978).Transformations: Growth and change in adult life. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. (1987).Emotion in psychotherapy: Affect, cognition, and the process of change. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartman, M., & Hasher, L. (1991). Aging and suppression: Memory for previously relevant information.Psychology and Aging, 6(4), 587–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., & Rypma, B. (1991). Age and inhibition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(1), 163–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),The Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heil, F. E., & Krampen, G. (1989). Action-theoretical approaches to the development of control orientations in the aged. In P. S. Fry (Ed.),Psychological perspectives of helplessness and control in the elderly (pp. 99–118). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, T. (1990).Constructive developmental analysis of autobiographical writing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, H. E., & Hohner, H. U. (1986). Occupational careers, work, and control. In M. M. Baltes & P. B. Baltes (Eds.),The psychology of control and aging (pp. 345–371). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1970).The crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958).The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence (A. Parsons & S. Milgram, Trans.). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, R. R. (1991). Reconceptualizing the nature of dialectical postformal operational thinking: The effects of affectively mediated social experiences. In J. D. Sinnott & J. C. Cavanaugh (Eds.),Bridging paradigms: Positive development in adulthood and cognitive aging (pp. 43–57). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1961).Psychology: The briefer course (G. Allport, Ed.). New York: Harper Torchbooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jedrzkiewicz, J. A. (1983).Adult development and mental effort: A neo-Piagetian experimental analysis. Unpublished master's thesis, York University, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J. (1991). Constructive processes in bilingualism and their cognitive growth effects. In E. Bialystok (Ed.),Language processing in bilingual children (pp. 193–221). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J., Fabian, V., & Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). Quantitative hardware stages that constrain language development.Human Development, 32, 245–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, J. W., & Rivera, R. J. (1965).Volunteers for learning: A study of the educational pursuits of American adults. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juckes, T. J. (1991). Equilibration and the learning paradox.Human Development, 34, 261–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knowles, M. S. (1980).The modern practice of adult education: From pedagogy to androgogy. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1986). Aging and models of control: The hidden costs of wisdom. In M. M. Baltes & P. B. Baltes (Eds.),The Psychology of control and aging (pp. 1–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1990). Models of knowledge and the organization of development. In M. L. Commons, C. Armon, L. Kohlberg, F. A. Richards, T. Grotzer, & J. Sinnott (Eds.),Adult development (Vol. 2, pp. 43–62). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1992). A neo-Piagetian perspective on adult cognitive development. In R. Sternberg & C. A. Berg (Eds.),Intellectual development (pp. 197–228). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe, M., & Bulka, D. (1989). Speaking about feelings: Conceptions of emotion across the life span.Psychology and Aging, 4(4), 425–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakim-Larson, J., DeVoe, M., & Schoeberlein, S. (1989). Emotions and self-regulation: A life span view.Human Development, 32, 279–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakim-Larson, J., & Hobart, C. J. (1987). Age, ego level, and the life-span development of coping and defense processes.Psychology and Aging, 2(3), 286–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, M. E. (1983). Perceptions of intellectual aging: Antecedent or consequence of intellectual functioning.Developmental Psychology, 19, 482–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J. (1989).Mindfulness. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion.American Psychologist, 46(4), 352–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, D. J., Darrow, C. N., Klein, E. B., Levinson, M. H., & McKee, B. (1978).The seasons of a man's life. New York: Ballantine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1951).Field theory in social science. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Long, H. B. (1983).Adult and continuing education: Responding to change. New York: Teacher's College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merriam, S. B., & Cunningham, P. M. (Eds.). (1989).Handbook of adult and continuing education. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mezirow, J. (1978). Perspective transformation.Adult Education, 28(2), 100–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mezirow, J. (1981). A critical theory of adult learning and education.Adult Education, 32(1), 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mezirow, J. (1985). Concept and action in adult education.Adult Education Quarterly, 35, 142–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mezirow, J. (1989). Transformation theory and social action: A response to Collard and Law.Adult Education Quarterly, 39(3), 169–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1986).Children's event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oddi, L. F. (1986). Development and validation of an instrument to identify self-directed continuing learners.Adult Education Quarterly, 36(2) 97–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oddi, L. F. (1987). Perspectives on self-directed learning.Adult Education Quarterly, 38(1), 21–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oddi, L. F., Ellis, A. J., & Roberson, J. E. A. (1990). Construct validation of the Oddi Continuing Learning Inventory.Adult Education Quarterly, 40(3), 139–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortega y Gasset, J. (1961).Meditations on Quixote. (E. Rugg & D. Marin, Trans.). New York: W. W. Norton. (Original work published 1914.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortega y Gasset, J. (1980).Sobre la razon historica. Madrid: Alianza Editorial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1969).Cognitive development and cognitive style: A general psychological integration. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Geneva.

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages.Acta Psychologica, 32, 301–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1976). Metasubjective problems of constructive cognition: Forms of knowing and their psychological mechanism.Canadian Psychological Review, 17(2), 110–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1980). Constructive problems for constructive theories: The current relevance of Piaget's work and a critique of information-processing simulation psychology. In R. Kluwe & H. Spada (Eds.),Developmental models of thinking (pp. 263–296). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1983). Growing into human maturity: Toward a metasubjective theory of adulthood stages. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim (Eds.),Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 117–156). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1984). Attentional, dialectic, and mental effort: Toward an organismic theory of life stages. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon, (Eds.),Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 182–215). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1987). Organismic processes for neo-Piagetian theories: A dialectical causal account of cognitive development.International Journal of Psychology, 22, 531–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). An organismic process model of Witkin's field dependence-independence. In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.),Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 36–70). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1990a). Reflections on life-span intelligence, consciousness, and ego development. In C. N. Alexander & E. Langer, (Eds.),Higher stages of human development: Perspectives on adult growth (pp. 258–285). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1990b). An essay on wisdom: Toward organismic processes that make it possible. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 244–278). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1991a). Emotions, development, and psychotherapy: A dialectical-constructivist perspective. In J. Safran & L. Greenberg (Eds.),Emotion, psychotherapy and change (pp. 302–335). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1991b). Commentary.Human Development, 34, 288–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., & Goodman, D. (1979). Intelligence and experience: A neo-Piagetian approach.Instructional Science, 8, 301–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., Goodman, D., Ammon, P., & Subelman, I. (1978). Piagetian theory and neo-Piagetian analysis as psychological guides in education. In J. M. Gallagher & J. A. Easley (Eds.),Knowledge and development (Vol. 2, pp. 243–289). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., & Ijaz, H. (1989). Mental capacity testing as a form of intellectual-developmental assessment. In R. Samuda, S. Kong, J. Cummings, J. Pascual-Leone, & J. Lewis (Eds.),Assessment and placement of minority students: A review for educators (pp. 143–171). Toronto: Hogrefe International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (1991). Psychological unit and its role in task analysis: A reinterpretation of object permanence. In M. Chandler & M. Chapman (Eds.),Criteria for competence: Controversies in the assessment of children's abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1968).The construction of reality in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). London: Routledge & K. Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Garcia, R. (1974).Understanding causality (D. & M. Miles, Trans.). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967).The child's conception of space. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachel, J. R. (1989). The social context of adult and continuing education. In S. B. Merriam & P. M. Cunningham (Eds.),Handbook of adult and continuing education (pp. 3–14). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, D. R., & Stirling, G. (1989). Cognitive social learning theory of control and aging, participatory control and the well-being of elderly persons. In P. S. Fry (Ed.),Psychological perspectives of helplessness and control in the elderly (pp. 217–258). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G. (February, 1992).Rocky roads to transfer. Final report, University of Arizona, Tucson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., & Globerson, T. (1987). Skill may not be enough: The role of mindfulness in learning and transfer.International Journal of Educational Research, 11, 623–637.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. (1989). Rocky roads to transfer: Rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon.Educational Psychologist, 24(2), 113–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977).Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attention, and general theory.Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, I. C., & Gatz, M. (1985). Age patterns in locus of control. In E. Palmore, E. W. Busse, G. L. Maddox, J. B. Nowlin, & I. C. Siegler, (Eds.),Normal aging III (pp. 259–267). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. (1981). Social experience in the development of representational thought: Distancing theory. In I. Sigel, D. Brodzinsky, & R. Golinkoff, (Eds.),New directions in Piagetian theory and practice (pp. 203–217). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. (1982). The relationship between parental distancing strategies and the child's cognitive behavior. In L. Laosa & I. Sigel (Eds.),Families as learning environments for children (pp. 47–86). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singley, M. K., & Anderson, J. R. (1989).The transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinnott, J. (1986). Prospective/intentional and incidental everyday memory: Effects of age and passage of time.Psychology and Aging, 1(2), 110–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Six, J. E. (1989). The generality of the underlying dimensions of the Oddi Continuing Learning Inventory.Adult Education Quarterly, 40(1), 43–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 1–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stankov, L. (1988). Aging, attention, and intelligence.Psychology and Aging, 3(1), 59–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. (Ed.). (1990).Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E., & Brunswick, F. (1935). The organism and the causal texture of the environment.Psychological Review, 42, 434–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vailliant, G. E. (1977).Adaptation to life. Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vuyk, R. (1981).Overview and critique of Piaget's genetic epistemology 1965–1980 (Vols. 1–2). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978).Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelinski, E. M., Gilewski, M., & Thompson, L. W. (1980). Do laboratory tasks relate to self-assessment of memory ability in the young and old? In L. W. Poon (Ed.),New directions in memory and aging: Proceedings of the George A. Talland Memorial Conference (pp. 519–544). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Pascual-Leone, J., Irwin, R.R. Noncognitive factors in high-road/low-road learning: I. Modes of abstraction in adulthood. J Adult Dev 1, 73–89 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02259674

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02259674

Key words

Navigation