Advertisement

Psychopharmacology

, Volume 110, Issue 3, pp 374–378 | Cite as

Effects of lorazepam upon recollective experience in recognition memory

  • H. Valerie Curran
  • John M. Gardiner
  • Rosalind I. Java
  • David Allen
Rapid Communications

Abstract

The effects of lorazepam (2 mg) and placebo upon recognition memory with and without conscious recollection were assessed in a cross-over study with normal volunteers. When recognising a word from study lists presented before and 1, 3 and 5 h after drug administration, subjects were required to indicate whether they could consciously recollect the word's prior occurrence or recognised it on the basis of “knowing”; in the absence of conscious recollection. Lorazepam only impaired word recognition which was accompanied by conscious recollection, and further, the level of this impairment correlated significantly with each of three different indices of subjects' arousal at the time of presentation of each list. Recognition in the absence of conscious recollection was not impaired but somewhat heightened by lorazepam, and these effects did not significantly relate to any index of arousal. These findings are interpreted as providing further support for the notion that recognition entails two distinct components, one based on contextual and associative information and related to conscious recollection, the other possibly based on a “traceless” perceptual or semantic memory system and related to feelings of knowing in the absence of conscious recollection. Implications are drawn for a contextual-encoding/retrieval account of lorazepam-induced amnesia.

Key words

Lorazepam Recognition memory Sedation Awareness 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Blaxton TA (submitted) The role of temporal lobes in recognizing nonverbal materials: Remembering versus knowingGoogle Scholar
  2. Bowers JS, Schacter DL (1990) Implicit memory and test awareness. J Exper Psychol [Learn Mem Cogn] 16:404–416Google Scholar
  3. Brown J, Lewis V, Brown MW, Horn G, Bowes JB (1982) A comparison between transient amnesia induced by two drugs (diazepam and lorazepam) and amnesia of organic origin. Neuropsychologia 20:55–70Google Scholar
  4. Brown MW, Brown J, Bowes J (1989) Absence of priming coupled with substantially preserved recognition in lorazepam induced amnesia. Q J Exp Psychol 41A:599–617Google Scholar
  5. Curran HV (1986) Tranquillising memories: a review of the effects of benzodiazepines on human memory. Biol Psychology 23:179–213Google Scholar
  6. Curran HV (1991) Benzodiazepines, memory and mood: a review. Psychopharmacology 105:1–8Google Scholar
  7. Curran HV (1992) Memory functions, alertness and mood of long-term benzodiazepine users: a preliminary investigation of the effects of a normal daily dose. J Psychopharmacol 6:69–75Google Scholar
  8. Curran HV, Birch B (1991) Differentiating the sedative, psychomotor and amnesic effects of benzodiazepines: a study with midazolam and the benzodiazepine antagonist, flumazenil. Psychopharmacology 103:519–523Google Scholar
  9. Curran HV, Gorenstein (in preparation) Differential effects of lorazepam and oxazepam on primingGoogle Scholar
  10. Curran HV, Schiwy W, Lader M (1987) Differential amnesic properties of benzodiazepines: a dose response comparison of two drugs with similar elimination half-lives. Psychopharmacology 92:358–364Google Scholar
  11. Curran HV, Schifano F, Lader M (1991) Models of memory dysfunction? A comparison of the effects of scopolamine and lorazepam on memory, psychomotor performance and mood. Psychopharmacology 103:83–90Google Scholar
  12. Danion J-M, Zimmermann M-A, Willard-Schroeder D, Grange D, Singer L (1989) Diazepam induces a dissociation between explicit and implicit memory. Psychopharmacology 99:238–243Google Scholar
  13. Fang JC, Hinrichs JV, Ghoneim MM (1987) Diazepam and memory: evidence for spared memory function. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 28:347–352Google Scholar
  14. Gardiner JM (1988) Functional aspects of recollective experience. Mem Cognit 16:309–313Google Scholar
  15. Gardiner JM, Java RI (1990) Recollective experience in word and non-word recognition. Mem Cognit 18:23–30Google Scholar
  16. Gardiner JM, Java RI (1991) Forgetting in recognition memory with and without recollective experience. Mem Cognt 19:617–623Google Scholar
  17. Gardiner JM, Java RI (1992) Recognising and remembering. In: Collins A, Conway MA, Gathercole SE, Morris PE (eds) Theories of memory, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, (in press)Google Scholar
  18. Gardiner JM, Parkin AJ (1990) Attention and recollective experience in recognition memory. Mem Cognit 18:579–583Google Scholar
  19. Ghoneim MM, Mewaldt SP (1990) Benzodiazepines and human memory: a review. Anaesthesiology 72:926–938Google Scholar
  20. Gregg VH, Gardiner JM (1991) Components of conscious awareness in a long-term modality effect. Br J Psychol 82:153–162Google Scholar
  21. Gregg VH, Gardiner JM, Greally T (in preparation) Modality effects in long-term recognition: when and why do they appear?Google Scholar
  22. Hashtroudi S, Parker ES, DeLisi LE, Wyatt RJ (1984) Intact retention in acute alcohol amnesia. J Exp Psychol [Human Learn] 10:156–163Google Scholar
  23. Hinrichs JV, Ghoneim MM, Mewaldt SP (1984) Diazepam and memory: retrograde facilitation produced by interference reduction. Psychopharmacology 84:158–162Google Scholar
  24. Jacoby LL, Dallas M (1981) On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. J Exper Psychol [Gen] 3:306–340Google Scholar
  25. Knopman D (1991) Unaware learning versus preserved learning in pharmacologic amnesia: similarities and differences. J Exp Psychol [Learn Mem Cogn] 17:1017–1029Google Scholar
  26. Kopelman MD, Corn TH (1988) Cholinergic “blockade” as a model for cholinergic depletion: a comparison of the memory deficits with those of Alzheimer-type dementia and the alcoholic Korsakoff syndrome. Brain 111:1079–1110Google Scholar
  27. Mogg K, Gardiner JM, Stavrou A, Golombok S (1992) Recollective experience and recognition memory for threat in clinical anxiety states. Bull Psychon Soc 30:109–112Google Scholar
  28. Parkin AJ, Walter B (1992) Recollective experience, normal aging, and frontal dysfunction. Psychol Aging (in press)Google Scholar
  29. Rajaram S (1992) The components of recollective experience: remembering and knowing. Mem CognitGoogle Scholar
  30. Richardson-Klavehn A, Bjork RA (1988) Measures of memory. Annu Rev Psychol 39:475–543Google Scholar
  31. Roediger HL, III, Weldon MS, Challis BH (1989) Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: a processing account. In: Roediger HL, III, Craik FIM (eds) Varieties of memory and consciousness: essays in honour of Endel Tulving Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ pp 3–41Google Scholar
  32. Schacter DL (1987) Implicit memory: History and current status. J Exper Psychol (Learn Mem Cogn) 13:501–518Google Scholar
  33. Smith ME (submitted) Neuropsychological manifestations of recollective experience during recognition memory judgementsGoogle Scholar
  34. Tulving E (1985) Memory and consciousness. Can Psychol 26:1–12Google Scholar
  35. Tulving E, Schacter DL (1990) Priming and human memory systems. Science 247:301–305Google Scholar
  36. Tulving E, Schacter DL, Stark HA (1982) Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory. J Exp Psychol [Learn Mem Cogn] 8:336–342Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • H. Valerie Curran
    • 1
  • John M. Gardiner
    • 2
  • Rosalind I. Java
    • 2
  • David Allen
    • 1
  1. 1.Institute of PsychiatryLondonUK
  2. 2.City UniversityLondonUK

Personalised recommendations