Intellectual ability and aggressive behavior in nonclinical-nonforensic males

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to test the relationship between IQ and physical aggression in a nonclinical-nonforensic male sample in a laboratory setting. Thirty males completed an abbreviated version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. Aggression was measured using a modified version of the Taylor reaction-time (RT) aggression paradigm in which subjects competed on a RT task and both received and delivered shocks to a fictitious opponent in provoking and nonprovoking conditions. Provocation conditions (High and Low) were defined by the intensity of shocks the subjects received. Results demonstrated strong inverse correlations between IQ and aggressive behavior under both High and Low Provocation conditions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Bernstein, S., Richardson, D., & Hammock, G. (1987). Convergent and discriminant validity of the Taylor and Buss measures of physical aggression.Aggressive Behavior, 13, 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Brooker, B. H., & Cyr, J. J. (1986). Tables for clinicians to use to convert WAIS-R short forms.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 982–986.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Buikhuisen, W. (1987). Cerebral dysfunctions and persistent juvenile delinquency. In S. A. Mednick, T. E. Moffitt, & S. A. Stack (Eds.),The causes of crime: New biological approaches (pp. 168–184). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  4. DeJong, J., Virkkunen, M., & Linnoila, M. (1992). Factors associated with recidivism in a criminal population.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 180, 543–550.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. DiLalla, L. F., & Gottesman, I. I. (1991). Biological and genetic contributors to violence: Widom's untold tale.Psychological Bulletin, 109, 125–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Elliott, F. A. (1992). Violence: The neurological contribution: An overview.Archives of Neurology, 49, 595–603.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Giancola, P., & Zeichner, A. (1994). Construct validity of a competitive reaction-time aggression paradigm.Aggressive Behavior (in press).

  8. Heilbrun, A. B. (1979). Psychopathy and violent crime.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 509–516.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hirschi, T., & Hindelang, M. J. (1977). Intelligence and delinquency: A revisionist review.American Sociological Review, 42, 571–587.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Hodges, K., & Plow, J. (1990). Intellectual ability and achievement in psychiatrically hospitalized children with conduct, anxiety, and affective disorders.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 589–595.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Kandel, E., Mednick, S. A., Kirkegaard-Sorensen, L., Hutchings, B., Knop, J., Rosenberg, R., & Schulsinger, F. (1988). IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for antisocial behavior.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 224–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. McGarvey, B., Gabrielli, W. F., Bentler, P. M., & Mednick, S. A. (1981). Rearing social class, education, and criminality: A multiple indicator model.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 354–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1988). IQ and delinquency: A test of the differential detection hypothesis.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 330–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Richters, J. E. (1993). Community violence and children's development: Toward a research agenda for the 1990s.Psychiatry, 56, 3–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Stark, R. (1975).Social problems. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Tarter, R. E., Hegedus, A. M., Winsten, N. E., & Alterman, A. I. (1984). Neuropsychological, personality, and familial characteristics of physically abused delinquents.Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23, 668–674.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Taylor, S. (1967). Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of provocation and the tendency to inhibit aggression.Journal of Personality, 35, 297–310.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Taylor, S., Gammon, C., & Capasso, D. (1976). Aggression as a function of the interaction of alcohol and threat.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 938–941.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1989). A prospective replication of the protective effects of IQ in subjects at high risk for juvenile delinquency.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 719–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wolfe, B. M., & Baron, R. A. (1971). Laboratory aggression related to aggression in naturalistic social situations.Psychonomic Science, 24, 193–194.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Yeudall, L., Fromm-Auch, D., & Davies, P. (1982). Neuropsychological impairment of persistent delinquency.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 170, 257–265.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amos Zeichner.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Giancola, P.R., Zeichner, A. Intellectual ability and aggressive behavior in nonclinical-nonforensic males. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 16, 121–130 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02232723

Download citation

Key words

  • IQ
  • aggression
  • provocation
  • Taylor paradigm