Skip to main content
Log in

How real is depressive realism? A question of scales and standards

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Depressive versus nondepressive realism and distortion were examined by relating both the direction and magnitude of social comparisons to objective standards. Using modified selection and comparative rating paradigms, results indicated that both dysphoric and nondysphoric subjects accurately identified whether they were worse-off, better-off, or similar to comparison targets (direction). However, subjects were not realistic in terms of the magnitude of the social comparison difference between themselves and another. In general, dysphorics' errors resulted in more unfavorable social comparisons relative to nondysphorics. Furthermore, results indicated that dysphorics and nondysphorics used different processes during social comparison: Dysphorics distorted perceptions of others whereas nondysphorics adjusted self-perceptions during social comparison. Our findings suggest that even when an objective standard is used, subjects' judgments can appear to be accurate or distorted depending on whether the data are analyzed according to direction or magnitude. Furthermore, negative distortion, not realism, is characteristic of even mildly depressed subjects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ackermann, R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1991). Is depressive realism real?Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 565–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albright, J. S., Alloy, L. B., Barch, D., & Dykman, B. M. (1993). Social comparison by dysphoric and nondysphoric college students: The grass isn't always greener on the other side.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17, 485–509.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988). Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.),Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 223–265). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1991).The Inference Questionnaire: Assessing bias and plausibility of pragmatic inferences about life events. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1993). Effects of social comparison direction, threat, and self-esteem on affect, self-evaluation, and expected success.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 708–722.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Tota, M. E. (1988). Context-dependent automatic processing in depression: Accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 925–939.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1989). The optimal margin of illusion. Special issue: Self-illusions: When are they adaptive?Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 176–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967).Depression: Clinical, experimental and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1976).Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).Cognitive therapy or depression. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation.Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buunk, B. P., Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., Van Yperen, N. W., & Dakof, G. A. (1990). The affective consequences of social comparison: Either direction has its ups and downs.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1238–1249.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981).Attention and self-regulation: A control theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clements, C. M., Alloy, L. B., Greenberg, M. S., & Kolden, G. (1987).Depression, anxiety and self-schemata: A test of Beck's content-specificity hypothesis. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972).A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes.Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, F. X. (1983). Self-focused attention and self-report validity: The “veridicality” hypothesis.Journal of Personality, 51, 517–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, F. X. (1986). Social comparison and depression: Company's effect on misery.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 140–148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1989). Effects of upward and downward social comparison on mood states.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 14–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1991). Downward comparison and coping with threat. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.),Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 317–345). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, F. X., & McCoy, S. B. (1991). Self-esteem, similarity, and reactions to active versus passive downward comparison.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 414–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goethals, G. R., Messick, D. M., & Allison, S. T. (1991). The uniqueness bias: Studies of constructive social comparison. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.),Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 149–176). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golin, S., Terrell, F., & Johnson, B. (1977). Depression and the illusion of control.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 440–442.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, M. S., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Depression versus anxiety: Processing of self- and other-referent information.Cognition and Emotion, 3, 207–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hemphill, K. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1991). Social comparison and their affective consequences: The importance of comparison dimension and individual difference variables.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 10, 372–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., Lumry, A. E., Cruet, D., & Sieber, W. (1987). Attentional processes in depressive disorders.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 351–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). The benefits of illusions, the threat of disillusionment, and the limitations of inaccuracy. Special issue: Self-illusions: When are they adaptive?Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 158–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuiper, N. A., & Derry, P. A. (1982). Depressed and nondepressed content self-reference in mild depressives.Journal of Personality, 50, 67–79.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuiper, N. A., McDonald, M. R., & Derry, P. A. (1983). Parameters of a depressive self-schema. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.),Psychological perspectives on the self. (Vol. 2, pp. 191–218). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, D. J., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (1984). The illusion of control for self and others in depressed and nondepressed college students.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 125–136.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pietromanaco, P. R., & Markus, H. (1985). The nature of negative thoughts in depression.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 799–807.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, J. B., Gibbons, F. X., Wicklund, R. A., Fazio, R. H., & Hood, R. (1977). Self-focused attention and self-report validity.Journal of Personality, 45, 514–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression.Psychological Bulletin, 102, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., Hamilton, J. C., Herring, F. H., & Greenberg, J. (1989). Depression, self-focused attention, and the negative memory bias.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 351–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., Holt, K., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-focused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative life events for self and others.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 994–1001.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarason, I. G., Johnson, J. H., & Siegel, J. M. (1978). Assessing the impact of life changes: Development of the Life Experiences Survey.Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 1–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V., & Dobson, K. S. (1992). Cognitive models of depression: Report from a consensus development conference.Psychological Inquiry, 3, 219–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swallow, S. R., & Kuiper, N. A. (1988). Social comparison and negative self-evaluations: An application to depression.Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 55–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnik, N., Crocker, J., & Alloy, L. B. (1983). Depression, social comparison and the false consensus effect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 688–699.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wills, T. A. (1986). Discussion remarks on social comparison theory.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 282–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wills, T. A. (1991). Similarity and self-esteem in downward comparison. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.),Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 51–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L. A. (1990). Does affect induce self-focused attention?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 899–908.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., Neale, J. M., Stone, A. A., & Rachmiel, T. B. (1990). Self-focused attention, coping responses, and distressed mood in everyday life.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1027–1036.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, J. V., & Taylor, K. L. (1991). Serving self-relevant goals through social comparison. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.),Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 23–49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Preparation of this manuscript was supported by a Loyola University of Chicago research stimulation grant to the first author (#389-05-000). The authors thank David Haaga, Emil Posavac, Robert Russell, Marc Wenzel, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions on earlier versions of this manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Albright, J.S., Henderson, M.C. How real is depressive realism? A question of scales and standards. Cogn Ther Res 19, 589–609 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02230515

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02230515

Key words

Navigation